Introduction

Human aging results in well-established declines in the brain’s macrostructure (Good et al. 2001; Smith et al. 2007). However, at least some age-related macrostructural declines can be attenuated by positive lifestyle variables. For example, a number of neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that specific lifestyle variables such as exercise, leisure activities, education, and occupation can have a positive impact on cortical structure in aging (Brayne et al. 2010; Colcombe et al. 2006; Sole-Padulles et al. 2009). These anatomically based structural benefits of positive lifestyle variables may contribute to individual differences in clinically manifested cognitive deficits (Stern et al. 1995a, b). For example, at the simplest level, larger brains can sustain more age-related pathology (Satz 1993). This phenomenon is commonly referred to as brain reserve and reflects quantitative differences in brain structure (i.e., brain volume, neuronal, or synaptic density) that allow some people to cope with neurodegeneration better than others (Stern 2009).

In recent years, it has become increasingly apparent that socioeconomic status (SES) is an important reserve variable in aging (Marengoni et al. 2011; Singh-Manoux et al. 2011). Variations in SES have been linked with marked differences in cognition and behavior across the life span (Hackman and Farah 2009; Hackman et al. 2010). High SES has been linked with increased exposure to cognitively stimulating environments compared to low SES (Hackman et al. 2010) and is associated with a lower risk of developing dementia (Stern et al. 1994; Valenzuela and Sachdev 2006). In contrast, low SES has been linked with greater exposure to life stressors and reduced brain volume in aging (Bartres-Faz et al. 2009; Sole-Padulles et al. 2009). SES is of strong theoretical interest to life span approaches to aging because it most likely continues to affect individuals across the duration of their employment career (into the sixth decade of life in most cases). As a result, it has now become standard practice to control for SES in studies of aging and dementia.

However, little is known about the potential relationship between SES and the brain’s white matter (WM) microstructure. A greater understanding of this potential relationship is important because microstructural integrity of cerebral WM is required for proper transmission of information between cortical regions. Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) can be used to explore potential SES–WM integrity relationships by evaluating the microstructural integrity of the brain’s WM tracts in vivo (Basser et al. 2000; Basser and Pierpaoli 1996; Le Bihan 2003). DTI is sensitive to the random motion of water molecules as they interact within tissues, thus reflecting characteristics of their immediate structural surroundings at a nominal 50-μ scale. Diffusion anisotropy can be measured by DTI by means of fractional anisotropy (FA), an index of overall tissue microstructural integrity (Pierpaoli et al. 1996).

While no studies have yet explored the relationship between WM microstructural integrity and SES per se, several recent studies have explored relationships between WM integrity and other reserve variables reflective of cognitive achievement (Arenaza-Urquijo et al. 2011; Teipel et al. 2009). For example, Teipel et al. (2009) explored the relationship between WM integrity and education in both healthy seniors and patients with clinically probable Alzheimer’s disease (AD) using a statistical parametric mapping approach. Interestingly, healthy seniors demonstrated a positive relationship between FA and education in WM tracts that showed an inverse relationship in AD participants. These findings suggest a relationship between reserve variables and WM integrity in healthy seniors.

However, there exist several important knowledge gaps in this area of study. The first concerns the relationship between the specific reserve variable of SES and WM integrity in healthy seniors. In particular, it is unknown whether SES is positively associated with WM integrity in regions that undergo age-related declines. WM regions that show significant age-related declines but are positively correlated with SES may reflect neuroprotective effects of SES in aging. In particular, those WM regions showing stronger SES–FA relationships in older adults than young adults may in part reflect the beneficial effects of continued exposure to high SES in aging.

If SES does offset age-related WM integrity declines in some regions, then it would be important to explore the potential biological mechanisms and functional significance of SES–WM integrity relationships in aging. An increased understanding of the neurobiological bases for potential SES–WM integrity relationships can be gained from the joint consideration of FA, radial diffusivity (DR), and axial diffusivity (DA) (Assaf and Pasternak 2008; Burzynska et al. 2010; Gold et al. 2012). For example, an SES–DR relationship would suggest that SES may help to preserve myelin integrity (Song et al. 2002, 2005), whereas an SES–DA relationship would suggest a relationship between SES and gross tissue characteristics (Sen and Basser 2005).

In terms of functional significance, it would be relevant to determine if SES-preserved WM integrity is associated with high performance on cognitive processes known to undergo marked age-related decline. One cognitive domain known to undergo significant age-related declines is working memory (Babcock and Salthouse 1990; Dobbs and Rule 1989; Park et al. 1997; Wiegersma and Meertse 1990), yet age-related declines in working memory may be attenuated by the relative preservation of WM integrity in individuals with high SES. The digit backwards (DB) span test was used to explore this possible relationship because it assesses the fundamental working memory function of maintaining information online (Groeger et al. 1999), a function which declines significantly with age (Baddeley 2000).

To address these issues, we first identified WM regions demonstrating significantly lower FA in seniors compared to young adults. We then identified SES–FA correlations within regions showing age-related declines. The FA metric represents a weighted average of DA and DR components of the diffusion tensor. We thus further characterized the neurobiological bases of significant SES–FA relationships by assessing if these relationships were primarily driven by SES–DR or SES–DA associations. Finally, we assessed the functional significance of maintained WM integrity by exploring the relationship between FA and working memory in regions where age-related WM integrity declines were offset by SES.

Methods

Participants

A total of 84 right-handed subjects participated in the present study. Participants were 40 (16 male) cognitively normal young adults (mean age = 33.3 years, SD = 4.27) and 44 (19 male) cognitively normal community dwelling seniors (mean age = 66.2 years, SD = 7.5). The young adult and senior groups did not differ in sex distribution [χ 2 (1) = 0.087, P = 0.768]. Informed consent was obtained from each participant under an approved University of Kentucky Institutional Review Board protocol. All participants were recruited from the local community through posted flyers and newspaper advertisements. Exclusionary criteria for the study included the following: a major head injury and/or concussion, stroke, a neurological or psychiatric disorder, untreated high blood pressure, hypercholesterolemia or diabetes, heart disease, the use of psychotropic medications (e.g., antidepressants), or the presence of metal fragments and/or metallic implants contraindicated for magnetic resonance imaging (MRI).

Demographic and cognitive tests

The Hollingshead two-factor index of social position.

The Hollingshead two-factor index of social position (ISP) was used as a measure of SES (Hollingshead 1958). The ISP is based on an individual’s occupation and highest level of formal education. It is calculated by assigning numeric values, from 1 to 7, to an individual’s occupation and education. Scores are then weighted by multiplying by 7 (occupation) and 4 (education). Values are then summed to produce a social index. For example, an individual with a Bachelor of Arts degree (numeric value = 2) who works as an administrative assistant (numeric value = 3) would have an ISP score of 29 ([2 × 4] + [3 × 7] = 29). Lower values represent higher earning occupations and more years of education. The two groups did not differ in mean SES (young adult group: M = 29.4, SD = 12.4; senior group: M = 28.4, SD = 11.0) [t(82) = 0.378, P < 0.707], and Levene’s test of homogeneity indicated that the two groups did not differ in SES variance [F(82) = 0.193, P = 0.662].

The Cattell culture fair intelligence test.

Because measures of SES are correlated with intelligence, and a relationship between intelligence quotient (IQ) and WM integrity has been reported (Chiang et al. 2011), we controlled for IQ using the Cattell culture fair (CCF) (Cattell and Cattell 1960). The CCF (scale 3) consists of 50 items and assesses inductive reasoning through the ability to perceive relationships in shapes and figures. Age-adjusted IQ scores were used because fluid intelligence has been shown to decline with age (Schretlen et al. 2000). The two groups did not differ in mean IQ (young adult group: M = 120.2, SD = 19.7; senior group: M = 123.8, SD = 21.1) [t(82) = −0.804, P < 0.424]. In addition, the CCF was used as a nuisance covariate in all analyses in order to control for within-group variance in IQ.

The digits span subtests of the Wechsler memory scale (Wechsler 1997).

Participants were read digit lists aloud at a rate of one per second with consistent tone and emphasis. Immediately after the digits were read, the participants were instructed to repeat each set of digits verbally in reverse order (digit backward; DB). Participants received two trials for each set of digits that ranged from 2 to 8 numbers. If the participant was unable to repeat all digits in a given set, the test was terminated. Totals were based on the number of trials that were accurately reported in the correct order. All scores were then adjusted for age.

Diffusion tensor imaging acquisition

Data were collected on a 3 Tesla Siemens TIM scanner at the University of Kentucky using an 8-channel head array coil. Whole-brain diffusion tensor images (40 contiguous 3-mm thick axial slices) were acquired with 36 non-collinear encoding directions (b = 1,000 s/mm2) and five images without diffusion weighting (b = 0 s/mm2, b0) using a double spin echo EPI sequence and the following parameters: repetition time = 6,900 ms, echo time (TE) = 84 ms, inversion time = 2,500 ms, flip angle = 90°, acquisition matrix = 128 × 128, field of view = 224 mm, in-plane resolution = 1.75 × 1.75 mm voxels. In addition, a double-echo gradient-echo sequence (TE1 = 5.19 ms, TE2 = 7.65 ms) with slice position and spatial resolution matching those of the EPI acquisition was used to map the spatial inhomogeneity of the B0 field.

Diffusion tensor imaging preprocessing and analysis

Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) data were preprocessed and analyzed using the Functional MRI of the Brain (FMRIB) software library (FSL v4.1.5). Each diffusion-weighted volume was corrected for motion and residual eddy current distortion using a 12-parameter affine alignment to the corresponding b0 image, via FMRIB’s Linear Image Registration Tool (FLIRT: http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). Images were then corrected for static field inhomogeneity distortions using B0 field maps. Brain masks were then generated from each b0 image using FMRIB’s brain extraction tool (BET v2.1) to exclude non-brain voxels from all subsequent processing (Smith et al. 2006). Next, FMRIB’s Diffusion Toolbox (FDT v2.0) was used to fit the diffusion tensor and calculate eigenvalues, fractional anisotropy (FA), axial diffusivity (DA), and radial diffusivity (DR).

Registration of FA images into MNI152 space and subsequent voxelwise analyses followed a series of procedures known as Tract-Based Spatial Statistics [TBSS v1.2; (Smith et al. 2006)], as described in detail in our previous work (Gold et al. 2010; Smith et al. 2010). Briefly, the initial step in this process was to remove likely outliers from the fitted tensor by eroding brain edge artifacts and zeroing the end slices. Next, all participants’ FA images were aligned to the FMRIB58_FA_1mm template using a nonlinear registration approach based on free-form deformations and B-Splines (Rueckert et al. 1999). The FA images were then affine registered and resampled to 1 × 1 × 1 mm MNI152 space. Transformations derived from the FA maps were then applied to the other diffusivity maps (DR and DA) for matched processing of all image volumes.

All MNI-transformed FA images were then averaged to create a mean FA image used to generate a common WM tract skeleton. An FA value of 0.2 was used to threshold the skeleton in order to minimize partial voluming effects after warping across subjects. Next, each participant’s spatially normalized FA image was projected onto the FA skeleton in order to account for residual misalignments between participants after the initial nonlinear registration. Finally, each subject’s DR and DA maps in MNI space were projected onto the common tract skeleton, using the pipeline for non-FA data provided by TBSS, which employs the projection vectors from each individual’s FA-to-skeleton transformation (Smith et al. 2006).

A main goal of the study was to determine if SES shows a neuroprotective effect in WM regions that undergo significant age-related declines. As a first step, a between-group comparison was performed to identify age-related changes in FA, with sex, IQ, and SES scores included as nuisance covariates. A permutation nonparametric test (using 5,000 permutations) was employed using a threshold-free cluster enhancement, and results were thresholded at P < 0.05 (corrected for multiple comparisons).

SES–FA correlations

As a second step, multiple regression analyses were performed to explore potential SES–FA relationships within regions showing age-related FA changes. Toward this end, an inclusion mask was first generated to include all voxels showing higher FA in the young group compared to the senior group. This inclusion mask was then resubmitted into the TBSS algorithm in order to search for SES–FA relationships within regions showing age-related FA declines. Age, sex, and IQ were included as covariates of no interest. Because the multiple regression analyses were restricted to (masked by) regions showing age-related FA declines, a statistical threshold of P < 0.001 (uncorrected) was employed with a cluster threshold of 20 voxels. For visualization purposes, all statistical maps were dilated using FSL’s tbss_fill.

Region of interest analyses

Results from the analyses described above demonstrated SES–FA correlations in the senior group in three frontal WM regions. Region of interest (ROI) analyses were conducted to further characterize the neurobiological bases and functional significance of these SES–FA correlations.

Component diffusivity analyses.

Multiple regression analyses were performed with SES as the dependent variable and age, sex, IQ, and mean DR and DA values from ROIs demonstrating SES–FA correlations as predictor variables. Standardized beta values were then assessed to determine the unique contribution of each independent variable. The largest standardized beta value indicates the independent variable that makes the strongest unique contribution to explaining the variance in SES in regions showing SES–WM integrity relationships in healthy seniors.

Correlation of FA and working memory.

The second ROI analysis was intended to explore relationships between working memory (i.e., digit backwards) and WM integrity in the three frontal ROIs that showed a correlation between SES and FA in the senior group. Partial correlations were used to determine the relationship between FA and working memory performance while controlling for sex and IQ.

Results

Demographic information for both groups is listed in Table 1. Results from the between-group comparison of FA are presented in Fig. 1. The senior group demonstrated significantly lower mean FA values (red) across a large portion, but not all, of the WM skeleton (green) after controlling for sex, IQ, and SES. The average age-related reduction in FA was significant across the entire WM skeleton F(1, 79) = 33.16, P < 0.001. There were no regions with significantly higher FA in the senior group compared to the young adult group.

Table 1 Demographic data
Fig. 1
figure 1

Age-related reductions in FA. Statistical map showing WM regions (a coronal, b sagittal, c transverse sections) where FA was significantly greater (red) in younger adults compared to seniors. The anatomic underlay used for illustration is the MNI152 T1-weighted 1-mm brain. The registered average FA skeleton is represented in green. d The average FA value across the entire WM skeleton is presented in the inset graph for both seniors and young adults. The average age-related reduction in FA was significant across the entire WM skeleton

SES–FA correlations

Figure 2 presents the results of the voxelwise multiple regression analysis between SES and FA in the senior group, and regression plots for the three regions consisting of ≥20 contiguous voxels (MNI coordinates are listed in Table 2). The correlation coefficients represent partial correlations after controlling for within-group variance in age, sex, and IQ. A positive correlation was observed between SES and FA in left superior frontal gyrus (SFG) WM, right SFG–WM, and the right anterior corona radiata (ACR) in the senior group. Specifically, the senior group demonstrated a positive correlation between SES and FA in left SFG–WM (r = 0.783, P < 0.001), right SFG–WM (r = 0.512, P < 0.001), and right ACR (r = 0.486, P < 0.001).

Fig. 2
figure 2

SES is correlated with FA in seniors. A positive correlation between SES and FA in the senior group was observed in a WM underlying the left superior frontal gyrus (SFG–WM), b WM underlying the right superior frontal gyrus SFG–WM, and c the right anterior coronal radiata (ACR). The numbers below each slice represent x coordinates in MNI space. The right panel shows scatter plots of the correlations between mean FA and SES in each WM region for each group (seniors = filled circles; young adults = open circles). Note: To render FA–SES regression plots more interpretable, participants’ SES values on the ISP were converted such that higher scores now reflected higher levels of SES (by subtracting each participant’s score from 100)

Table 2 Location and size of white matter regions showing selective positive SES–FA correlations in the senior group

In contrast, in the younger adult group, no relationship was observed between SES and FA in the three frontal regions showing correlations in the senior group: left SFG–WM ROI (r = 0.147, P = 0.193), right SFG–WM ROI (r = −0.100, P = 0.279), or right ACR ROI (r = −0.187, P = 0.134). In addition, multiple regression analysis conducted across the brain’s WM (i.e., not restricted to regions showing age-related FA declines) revealed no regions showing a positive SES–FA relationship in the younger group.

Region of interest analyses

Component diffusivity analyses.

The total SES variance explained by the model was 63.3 % in the left SFG–WM ROI (R = 0.796, F(5, 38) = 13.13, P < 0.001), 35.3 % in the right SFG–WM ROI (R = 0.549, F(5, 38) = 4.14, P < 0.004), and 30.2 % in the right ACR ROI (R = 0.549, F(5, 38) = 3.28, P < 0.015). DR made the strongest contribution to the variance in SES in each of these three regions [left SFG–WM ROI (beta = −1.30, P < 0.001), right SFG–WM ROI (beta = −0.57, P = 0.006), and right ACR ROI (beta = −0.62, P = 0.001)]. The unique contribution of both DR and DA are listed in Table 3. Scatter plots illustrating the relationship between DR and SES in each ROI are present in Fig. 3. The correlation coefficients represent partial correlations after controlling for within-group variance in age, sex, and IQ.

Table 3 Unique contributions of radial and axial diffusivity measures in predicting SES
Fig. 3
figure 3

Relationship between SES and DR in regions showing a positive SES–FA correlation in seniors. Regression plots show the negative relationship between SES and DR in a WM underlying the left superior frontal gyrus (SFG–WM), b WM underlying the right superior frontal gyrus SFG–WM, and c the right anterior coronal radiata (ACR)

Correlation of FA and working memory.

Figure 4 presents the correlations between FA and working memory in regions that showed a positive SES–FA relationship in healthy seniors. The correlation coefficients represent partial correlations after controlling for within-group variance in age, sex, and IQ. A positive correlation was observed between FA and DB in the left (r = 0.323, P = 0.019) and right (r = 0.323, P = 0.019) SFG–WM ROIs. Similarly, an FA–DB trend was observed in the right ACR ROI (r = 0.237, P = 0.066). For the young adults, no correlation was observed between FA and DB in the left SFG ROI (r = 0.060, P = 0.363), the right SFG ROI (r = −0.115, P = 0.249), or the right ACR ROI (r = −0.110, P = 0.259).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Relationship between FA and working memory (digit backward) in regions showing a positive SES–FA correlation in seniors. The left (a) and right (b) SFG–WM regions showing a positive correlation between SES and FA also showed a positive FA–DB correlation. The right panel shows scatter plots of the correlations between mean FA and DB

Discussion

The present study represents the first systematic exploration of the relationship between socioeconomic status (SES) and WM microstructural integrity relevant to normal aging. Strengths of our study include the use of a TBSS method with rigorously validated registration methods, exploration of three main components of the diffusion tensor (FA/DR/DA), and the control of age, sex, and IQ variables in all analyses. Seniors demonstrated significantly lower FA across widespread portions of WM. However, high SES offset age-related FA declines in several frontal WM regions. In addition, the relative preservation of WM integrity in these regions was positively associated with working memory span. The details of these findings are discussed below.

We observed diffuse age-related reductions in FA in frontal, parietal, occipital, and temporal WM, consistent with previous results (Abe et al. 2002; Madden et al. 2004; O'Sullivan et al. 2001; Pfefferbaum et al. 2000). Within regions showing age-related WM integrity declines, we observed positive SES–FA relationships in several frontal regions in the senior group. Specifically, in the senior group, positive SES–FA correlations were observed in bilateral portions of WM underlying the superior frontal gyri (SFG–WM) and in the right anterior corona radiata (ACR). The location of the observed SES–FA relationship within frontal regions is consistent with a view that high levels of SES are associated with an increasing emphasis on high-level decision making, planning, and goal-directed behavior (Andersen et al. 2004; Kristensen et al. 2002). The positive SES–WM integrity relationship in the senior group could be the result of biological mechanisms (i.e., higher WM integrity leads to higher SES) or environmental mechanisms (i.e., SES is neuroprotective of WM integrity). These possibilities are not mutually exclusive and may both be at work in some interactive fashion.

To identify potential neuroprotective effects, we included a young group that was matched with the senior group for mean SES, SES variance, IQ, and gender. Despite this matching, the observed SES–FA correlations in frontal WM were selective for the senior group. The unique correlations in the senior group raise the possibility that high SES may neuroprotect some age-related frontal WM integrity declines. The anatomical specificity of the observed SES–frontal relationship may in part reflect that frontal WM tracts, because they are especially vulnerable to age-related declines (Head et al. 2004; O'Sullivan et al. 2001; Salat et al. 2005a, b), also carry greater chance of incurring protective benefits from high SES than other regions.

Although the neurobiological mechanisms contributing to a potential SES–WM integrity relationship are still unclear, cognitive stimulation, and the subsequent activation of cortical neurons, may play a significant role because axon myelination appears to be triggered by neural activity (Bradl and Lassmann 2010; Gyllensten and Malmfors 1963; Omlin 1997). Support for such a mechanism comes from studies exploring the effects of cognitive stimulation (cognitive training) on WM integrity (Engvig et al. 2011; Lovden et al. 2010). For example, Lovden et al. (2010) demonstrated that cognitive training in young adults and healthy seniors results in increased WM integrity in frontal commissural tracts, suggesting that regular cognitive stimulation may help contribute to dynamic changes in WM microstructural integrity at any age.

In partial support of this view, joint consideration of the other major components of the diffusion tensor showed that the positive SES–FA correlations in the left SFG–WM, right SFG–WM, and right ACR were primarily explained by negative SES–DR correlations. Interestingly, similar findings have been reported in frontal tracts in association with a different kind of reserve variable, cardiorespiratory fitness (Johnson et al. 2012). The convergence of these findings raises the possibility that maintenance of myelin integrity within frontal WM tracts may serve as one common mechanism of brain reserve. However, future longitudinal research will be required to determine if the adoption of positive lifestyle variables can attenuate the rate of myelin loss in cognitively normal seniors.

Finally, to determine the potential functional significance of maintained frontal WM integrity in aging, we explored the relationship between working memory (i.e., DB) and WM integrity in frontal regions showing positive SES–FA correlations in the senior group. We observed a positive relationship between microstructural integrity and DB in WM underlying the left and right SFG. The observed association has high face validity because WM underlying bilateral portions of the SFG include tract connections with the left and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, brain regions that play central roles in working memory function (D'Esposito et al. 1995; Salmon et al. 1996).

The present study has several caveats that highlight the need for future work in this field. First, the cross-sectional design used in this study is unable to provide definitive information on cause-and-effect relationships between SES and WM integrity. Furthermore, the cross-sectional design did not allow us to account for potential cohort effects (Deary et al. 2003). Second, SES is considered to be a proxy indicator for other lifestyle variables known to contribute to reserve (such as physical activity, access to health care, and dietary habits). These variables could contribute to the present SES correlations and should be explored in future work. Additionally, our younger and older group samples demonstrated a relatively high mean IQ. Future work will be required to determine if our findings generalize to groups with average IQs. Third, the underlying biophysical properties derived from the diffusion tensor still need to be elucidated in humans. Although animal model studies have established a link between demyelination and DR (Song et al. 2002, 2005), this link is not as well established in humans (Wheeler-Kingshott and Cercignani 2009). Lastly, future studies should explore the contribution of genetic factors, such as the presence of apolipoprotein-E4 allele, as genetics can interact with environmental variables to influence cortical structure (Anttila et al. 2004; den Heijer et al. 2004; Dufouil et al. 2000).

In conclusion, our results suggest a potential neuroprotective effect of SES on frontal WM integrity. The positive SES–FA correlations observed were primarily driven by negative DR–SES correlations, suggesting that SES may buffer age-related changes in myelin. The functional significance of high WM integrity in these frontal regions was demonstrated through positive correlations with working memory span. These findings suggest that SES may help maintain frontal WM integrity and working memory function in aging. On the flip side, our results draw further attention to the fact that disparities in SES affect not only one’s status in society but also the health of the aging brain.