Skip to main content
Log in

What counts as a flexible representational choice? An evaluation of students’ representational choices to solve linear function problems

  • Published:
Instructional Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study evaluated students’ representational choices while they solved linear function problems. Eighty-six secondary-school students solved problems under one choice condition, where they chose a table, a formula, or both to solve each problem, and two no-choice conditions, where one of these representations was forced upon them. Two conceptualisations of representational flexibility were used: a groupwise conceptualisation, where group-based measures of students’ fluency with the different representations were used to determine which representational choices could be considered flexible, and an individualised conceptualisation, where each individual student’s fluency with each of the representations to solve each problem type was taken into account when determining which choices could be considered flexible for that particular student. A strong correlation between groupwise flexibility and choice condition accuracy, and an even stronger correlation between individualised flexibility and choice condition accuracy were found. The implications for research and instruction are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The choice/no-choice method was borrowed from the literature on strategy choice. This method was initially used to analyse students’ strategy choices in multiplication tasks (Siegler and Lemaire 1997), and later on in other tasks from various cognitive domains (for an overview see Luwel et al. 2009). Given the strong link that exists between strategies and representations (Borba and Confrey 1996; Tabachneck et al. 1994a), the method has recently been applied to the field of mathematical problem solving with external representations (Acevedo Nistal et al. 2010).

  2. Siegler and Lemaire (1997) speak about ‘adaptivity’, and not flexibility. Some authors do not consider these terms synonymous, but for the sake of simplicity we will do so in this article. A discussion concerning the differences between these terms can be found in Verschaffel et al. (2009).

  3. For purposes of readability, statistical details are only included in text for the main effects and interaction effects. For the numerous contrast analyses (e.g., pairwise comparisons between grades, problem types, etc.), we only report whether differences are statistically significant (p < .05) or not, without mentioning each time χ² scores, degrees of freedom and exact p values.

References

  • Acevedo Nistal, A., Van Dooren, W., Clarebout, G., Elen, J., & Verschaffel, L. (2009). Conceptualising, investigating, and stimulating representational flexibility in mathematical problem solving and learning: A critical review. ZDM—The International Journal of Mathematics Education, 41(1), 627–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Acevedo Nistal, A., Van Dooren, W., Clarebout, G., Elen, J., & Verschaffel, L. (2010). Representational flexibility in linear-function problems: A choice/no-choice study. In L. Verschaffel, E. De Corte, J. Elen, & T. De Jong (Eds.), Use of representations in reasoning and problem solving: Analysis and improvement (pp. 74–93). UK: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainsworth, S. (1999). Designing effective multi-representational learning environments. University of Nottingham SRC Tech report no. 58.

  • Bieda, K. N., & Nathan, M. J. (2009). Representational disfluency in algebra: Evidence from student gestures and speech. ZDM—The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 41, 637–650. doi:10.1007/s11858-009-0198-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borba, M. C., & Confrey, J. (1996). A student’s construction of transformations of functions in a multiple representational environment. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 31, 319–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cox, R., & Brna, P. (1995). Supporting the use of external representations in problem solving: The need for flexible learning environments. Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education, 6, 239–302.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, T., Ainsworth, S., Dobson, M., van der Hulst, A., Levonen, J., Reimann, P., et al. (1998). Acquiring knowledge in science and math: The use of multiple representations in technology based learning environments. In M. W. van Someren, P. Reimann, H. P. A. Boshuizen, & T. de Jong (Eds.), Learning with multiple representations (pp. 9–40). Amsterdam: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dienes, Z. P. (1960). Building up mathematics. Great Britain: Anchor Press, Hutchinson Educational.

    Google Scholar 

  • diSessa, A. A., & Sherin, B. L. (2000). Meta-representation: An introduction. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 19, 385–398. doi:10.1016/S0732-3123(01)00051-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duval, R. (2002). The cognitive analysis of problems of comprehension in the learning of mathematics. Mediterranean Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 1(2), 1–16. doi:10.1007/s10649-006-0400-z.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, T. (1992). On the development of a sense for functions. In E. Dubinsky & G. Harel (Eds.), The concept of function. Aspects of epistemology and pedagogy (pp. 153–174). Washington, DC: The Mathematical Association of America.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elia, I., Panaoura, A., Eracleous, A., & Gagatsis, A. (2007). Relations between secondary pupils’ conceptions about functions and problem solving in different representations. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 5, 533–556. doi:10.1007/s10763-006-9054-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1980). Verbal reports as data. Psychological Review, 87, 215–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Even, R. (1998). Factors involved in linking representations of functions. Journal of Mathematical Behaviour, 17, 105–121. doi:10.1016/S0732-3123(99)80063-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gagatsis, A., & Shiakalli, M. (2004). Ability to translate from one representation of the concept of function to another and mathematical problem solving. Educational Psychology, 24, 645–657. doi:10.1080/0144341042000262953.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilmore, D. J., & Green, T. R. G. (1984). Comprehension and recall of miniature programs. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 21, 31–48. doi:10.1016/S0020-7373(84)80037-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldin, G. A. (2003). Representation in school mathematics: A unifying research perspective. In J. Kilpatrick, W. Gary Martin, & D. Schifter (Eds.), A research companion to principles and standards for school mathematics (pp. 275–285). Reston, NJ: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grawemeyer, B. (2006). Evaluation of ERST—an external representation selection tutor. In D. Barker-Plummer, R. Cox, & N. Swoboda (Eds.), Diagrammatic representation and inference: 4th international conference, Diagrams 2006, Stanford, CA, USA, June 28–30, 2006, Proceedings (pp. 154–167). New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grüber, H., Graf, M., Mandl, H., Renkl, A., & Stark, R. (1995, August). Fostering applicable knowledge by multiple perspectives and guided problem solving. Paper presented at the 6th conference of the European Association for Research on Learning and Instruction, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.

  • Herman, M. (2007). What students choose to do and have to say about use of multiple representations in college algebra. Journal of Computers in Mathematics and Science Teaching, 26, 27–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hitt, F. (1998). Difficulties in the articulation of different representations linked to the concept of function. Journal of Mathematical Behaviour, 17, 123–134. doi:10.1016/S0732-3123(99)80064-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaput, J. (1992). Technology and mathematics education. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 515–556). New York, NY: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larkin, J. H., & Simon, H. A. (1987). Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth ten thousand words. Cognitive Science, 11, 65–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luwel, K., Onghena, P., Torbeyns, J., Schillemans, V., & Verschaffel, L. (2009). Strengths and weaknesses of the choice/no-choice method in research on strategy use. European Psychologist, 14, 351–362. doi:10.1027/1016-9040.14.4.351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martínez, M., & Brizuela, B. (2006). A third grader’s way of thinking about linear function tables. Journal of Mathematical Behaviour, 25, 285–298. doi:10.1016/j.jmathb.2006.11.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Monk, S. (2003). Representation in school mathematics: Learning to graph and graphing to learn. In J. Kilpatrick, W. Gary Martin, & D. Schifter (Eds.), A research companion to principles and standards for school mathematics (pp. 250–262). Reston, NJ: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nathan, M. J. (1992). Interactive depiction of mathematical constraints can increase students word problem solving. In W. Geeslin & K. Grahm (Eds.), Proceedings of the sixteenth psychology of mathematics education (PME) conference (Vol. 2, pp. 160–169). Durham, NH: Program Committee of the 16th PME.

  • National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2000). Principles and standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schnotz, W., & Bannert, M. (2003). Construction and interference in learning from multiple representations. Learning and Instruction, 13, 141–156. doi:10.1016/S0959-47520200017-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S., & Lemaire, P. (1997). Older and younger adults’ strategy choices in multiplication: Testing predictions of ASCM using the choice/no-choice method. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 71–92. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.126.1.71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparrow, J. A. (1989). Graphical displays in information systems: Some data properties influencing the effectiveness of alternative formats. Behaviour & Information Technology, 8, 43–56. doi:10.1080/01449298908914537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tabachneck, H. J. M., Koedinger, K. R., & Nathan, M. J. (1994a). Towards a theoretical account of strategy use and sense making in mathematical problem solving. In A. Ram & K. Eiselt (Eds.), 16th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 836–841). Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabachneck, H. J. M., Leonardo, A. M., & Simon, H. A. (1994b). How does an expert use a graph? A model of visual and verbal inferencing in economics. In A. Ram & K. Eiselt (Eds.), Proceedings of the 16th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 842–847). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verschaffel, L., Luwel, K., Torbeyns, J., & Van Dooren, W. (2009). Conceptualizing, investigating, and enhancing adaptive expertise in elementary mathematics education. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 24, 335–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verschaffel, L., Torbeyns, J., De Smedt, B., Luwel, K., & Van Dooren, W. (2007). Strategy flexibility in children with low achievement in mathematics. Educational & Child Psychology, 24(2), 16–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vessey, I. (1991). Cognitive fit: A theory-based analysis of the graph versus tables literature. Decision Sciences, 22, 219–240. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5915.1991.tb00344.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warner, L. B. (2005). Behaviors that indicate mathematical flexible thought. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, New Brunswick.

  • Wickens, C. D., & Andre, A. D. (1990). Proximity compatibility and information display: Effects of color, space and objectness on information integration. Human Factors, 32, 61–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yerushalmy, M. (1991). Student perceptions of aspects of algebraic function using multiple representation software. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 7, 42–57. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2729.1991.tb00223.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yerushalmy, M. (1997). Designing representations: Reasoning about functions of two variables. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 28, 431–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yerushalmy, M. (2006). Slower algebra students meet faster tools: Solving algebra word problems with graphing software. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 37, 356–387.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was partially supported by Grant GOA 2006/01 “Developing adaptive expertise in mathematics education” from the Research Fund K.U. Leuven, Belgium, and the Grant G063709N “Representational adaptivity in mathematical thinking and learning: analysis and improvement” from the Fund for Scientific Research Flanders.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ana Acevedo Nistal.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Acevedo Nistal, A., Van Dooren, W. & Verschaffel, L. What counts as a flexible representational choice? An evaluation of students’ representational choices to solve linear function problems. Instr Sci 40, 999–1019 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-011-9199-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-011-9199-9

Keywords

Navigation