Skip to main content
Log in

The efficiency of crackdowns: a lab-in-the-field experiment in public transportations

  • Published:
Theory and Decision Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The concentration of high-frequency controls in a limited period of time (“crackdowns”) constitutes an important feature of many law-enforcement policies around the world. In this paper, we offer a comprehensive investigation on the relative efficiency and effectiveness of various crackdown policies using a lab-in-the-field experiment with real passengers of a public transport service. We introduce a novel game, the daily public transportation game, where subjects have to decide, over many periods, whether to buy or not a ticket knowing that there might be a control. Our results show that (a) concentrated crackdowns are less effective and efficient than random controls; (b) prolonged crackdowns reduce fare-dodging during the period of intense monitoring but induce a burst of fraud as soon as they are withdrawn; (c) pre-announced controls induce more fraud in the periods without control. Overall, we also observe that real fare-dodgers fraud more in the experiment than non-fare-dodgers.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. These studies either did not explicitly compare the efficiency and effectiveness of a crackdown policy to an alternative mechanism of crime prevention such as random controls (Di Tella and Schargrodsky 2003, 2004; Banuri and Eckel 2015) or they vary at the same time other things than just the concentration of controls (Dai et al. 2015; Kastlunger et al. 2009). For instance, Dai et al. (2015) also vary the total number of audits, while Kastlunger et al. (2009) indirectly change subjects’ expectation of an audit by informing them in advance of the overall probability of being audited.

  2. In the city where this study was conducted, the price of the ticket was, at the time, €1.70.

  3. See, for example, Dai et al. (2015) for a discussion on related studies.

  4. To strengthen the link of our experiment with the real world, we also frame the experimental setting in terms of the natural environment where these subjects take decisions about public transportation. This also facilitated the comprehension of the instructions for participants who possessed very different socio-economic and educational backgrounds.

  5. Lando and Shavell (2004) and Kleiman and Kilmer (2009) are more interested on the implications of concentrating sanctions on a subset of violators rather than a period of time or geographical area.

  6. We acknowledge that there are a few studies in the literature on tax evasion that investigate the effect of sending threat-of-audit letters (e.g. Slemrod et al. 2001; Kleven et al. 2011; Fiorio et al. 2013). What is usually found is that informing people about the audit activity has a positive and statistically significant effect on tax compliance. More relevant for us are the experimental studies of Alm and McKee (2006) and Alm et al. (2009) on tax evasion. Alm and McKee (2006) show experimentally that, when some individuals are informed in advance of an audit while others are informed that they will not be audited, the pre-announcement increases the compliance of those informed they will be audited but the overall compliance falls. Alm et al. (2009) consider different ways in which information regarding the audit activity can be transmitted ex-ante and ex-post to the taxpayers. They ran sessions where the audit probability was pre-announced and sessions where it was not, and found that compliance was on average lower in the former. However, differently from our experiment, both Alm and McKee (2006) and Alm et al. (2009) do not consider periods of durable controls (i.e., crackdowns). In addition, they only focus on the immediate effects of pre-announcement while we are interested in both the immediate and long-term effects of crackdowns.

  7. We asked subjects to make several decisions under different audit probabilities and fines.

  8. The order in which subjects made their choices was kept fixed to facilitate decision-making and avoid confusion.

  9. Giving an initial endowment of cash to the subjects to prevent a loss is a standard practice in experimental economics to avoid bankruptcy (see Friedman and Sunder 1994). We acknowledge that this initial endowment may result in a house money effect that may affect the extent to which people are willing to cheat in the experiment. However, as the main aim of this study is to compare the efficiency and effectiveness of different policies across treatments, if this house money effect exists, it should be similar across treatments. Thus, it should not bias our between-treatment comparisons.

  10. We however refrain from using strong terminology which could lead to experimenter demand effects.

  11. This was also equivalent to the real cost of using a public transport in the French city where the experiment took place.

  12. The reason why we started the crackdown in day 3 and day 33 is that we did not want subjects to easily learn the occurrence of an audit.

  13. If the audit probability was known, rational and risk-neutral agents should always choose to buy a ticket since the payoff would be higher than not to buy a ticket.

  14. To maximize the representativeness of our sample pool, we adopted a stratified random procedure of recruitment based on the information collected by the local public transport agency TCL-Sytral in a 2014 compulsory survey. Compared to this representative sample, our subject pool differs mainly in terms of age (we did not recruit minors, we slightly over-represented passengers from 18 to 24 years old, and under-represented passengers older than 60) and status (we over-represented unemployed people and under-represented white collars and retirees). See Dai et al. (2016) for a detailed discussion on this point. It is, of course, possible that our sample, compared to the reference statistical population, differed in other unobservable dimensions (e.g., impatient people might be less likely to participate in this study), for which, however, we do not have a control.

  15. After having been punched, a ticket is valid for 1 h. Hence, it was entirely legitimate and credible to compensate a ticket-holding subject with a new ticket for the time spent in the lab.

  16. In the questionnaire, they were asked to report how often (from 0 to 10) they travel without a valid ticket out of 10 trips.

  17. An alternative procedure would be that of relying only on one of the three measures. The risk is of course that of underestimating the number of fare-dodgers.

  18. The lab was also near the tram and bus stop (about 100 m from the stop).

  19. The opposite is likely to be true, that is subjects who self-reported as non-fare-dodgers were actually fare-dodgers.

  20. We believe these are more likely to be occasional fare-dodgers.

  21. These figures are consistent with data from the local public transport company (see, e.g., Keolis 2014), and other studies conducted in comparable countries (Bucciol et al. 2013). Our high percentages are also due to the fact that we excluded people with a monthly pass from our experiment.

  22. The number of actual fare-dodgers varies between 61 % (C_Long1) to 79 % (RR).

  23. The higher the value, the less risk averse the subject is.

  24. The results remain qualitatively the same if we drop these demographic variables.

  25. A crackdown ran from day 3 (33) until day 8 (38) but its effect operated from day 4 (34) until day 9 (39). In other words, the effect of a crackdown was 1-day delayed. This is because subjects were not informed in advance of a crackdown. There is however an exception: in the CC_A treatment, the control was pre-announced and, therefore, the effect and execution of a crackdown coincided. For this treatment, crackdown days are 3–8 and 33–38. This is taken into account in Fig. 3.

  26. We also find that older subjects, as well as educated subjects, are less likely to fraud in our game. These effects should be however interpreted with caution. Note in fact that these demographics correlate with being a fraudster in the field. So part of their effects is captured by the variable “Fare-dodgers”.

References

  • Alm, J., & McKee, M. (2006). Audit certainty, audit productivity, and taxpayer compliance. National Tax Journal, 59(4), 801–816.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., Jackson, B. R., & McKee, M. (2009). Getting the word out: Enforcement information dissemination and compliance behavior. Journal of Public Economics, 93(3–4), 392–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Banuri, S., & Eckel, C. (2015). Cracking down on bribery. Social Choice and Welfare, 45(3), 579–600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonfanti, G., & Wagenknecht, T. (2010). Human factors reduce aggression and fare evasion. Public Transport International, 59, 28–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bucciol, A., Landini, F., & Piovesan, M. (2013). Unethical behavior in the field: Demographic characteristics and beliefs of the cheater. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 93, 248–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cardenas, J. C., & Carpenter, J. (2013). Risk attitudes and economic well-being in Latin America. Journal of Development Economics, 103, 52–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cour des Comptes. (2016). La lutte contre la fraude dans les transports urbains en Île-de-France: un échec collectif, in Rapport public annuel 2016, pp. 537–577.

  • Dai, Z. (2016). Endogenous Crackdowns: Theory and Experimental Evidence. Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=2799955

  • Dai, Z., Hogarth, R. M., & Villeval, M. C. (2015). Ambiguity on audits and cooperation in a public goods game. European Economic Review, 74, 146–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dai, Z., Galeotti, F., & Villeval, M.C. (2016). Cheating in the Lab Predicts Fraud in the Field: An Experiment in Public Transportations, Bonn: IZA Discussion Paper No. 9702.

  • Di Tella, R., & Schargrodsky, E. (2003). The role of wages and auditing during a crackdown on corruption in the city of Buenos Aires. The Journal of Law and Economics, 46(1), 269–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Di Tella, R., & Schargrodsky, E. (2004). Do police reduce crime? Estimates using the allocation of police forces after a terrorist attack. American Economic Review, 94(1), 115–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckel, C. C., & Grossman, P. J. (2008). Men, women and risk aversion: Experimental evidence. Handbook of Experimental Economics Results, 1, 1061–1073.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckel, C. C., Grossman, P. J., Johnson, C. A., de Oliveira, A. C., Rojas, C., & Wilson, R. K. (2012). School environment and risk preferences: Experimental evidence. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 45(3), 265–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eeckhout, J., Persico, N., & Todd, P. E. (2010). A theory of optimal random crackdowns. American Economic Review, 100(3), 1104–1135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farrell, G., & Thorne, J. (2005). Where have all the flowers gone? Evaluation of the Taliban crackdown against opium poppy cultivation in Afghanistan. International Journal of Drug Policy, 16(2), 81–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiorio, C., Iacus S., & Santoro, A. (2013). Taxpaying response of small firms to an increased probability of audit: some evidence from Italy, University of Milan Bicocca Working Paper 251.

  • Fischbacher, U., & Föllmi-Heusi, F. (2013). Lies in disguise: An experimental study on cheating. Journal of the European Economic Association, 11(3), 525–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, D., & Sunder, S. (1994). Experimental methods: A primer for economists. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fréchette, G. R., & Schotter, A. (Eds.). (2015). Handbook of experimental economic methodology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, G. W., & List, J. A. (2004). Field experiments. Journal of Economic Literature, 42(4), 1009–1055.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kastlunger, B., Kirchler, E., Mittone, L., & Pitters, J. (2009). Sequences of audits, tax compliance, and taxpaying strategies. Journal of Economic Psychology, 30, 405–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keolis. (2014). “Fraude: comment lutter?” Keo’, February. Available online at http://www.keolis.com/fileadmin/documents/medias/publications/keo/KEO1402.pdf. Accessed 13 October 2015.

  • Khan, U., & Dhar, R. (2006). Licensing effect in consumer choice. Journal of Marketing Research, 43(2), 259–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleiman, M. (2009). When brute force fails: How to have less crime and less punishment. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kleiman, M., & Kilmer, B. (2009). The dynamics of deterrence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(34), 14230–14235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleven, H. J., Knudsen, M. B., Kreiner, C. T., Pedersen, S., & Saez, E. (2011). Unwilling or unable to cheat?: evidence from a tax audit experiment in Denmark. Econometrica, 79(3), 651–692.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lando, H., & Shavell, S. (2004). The advantage of focusing law enforcement effort. International Review of Law and Economics, 24(2), 209–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lazear, E. (2006). Speeding, terrorism, and teaching to the test. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 121(3), 1029–1061.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazar, N., & Ariely, D. (2006). Dishonesty in everyday life and its policy implications. Journal of Public Policy and Marketing, 25, 117–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moeller, K. K. (2009). Police crackdown on Christiania in Copenhagen. Crime, Law and Social Change, 52(4), 337–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, C. B. (1989). Economic crimes in the People’s Republic of China: A swinging door policy. American University International Law Review, 5, 161–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, L. W. (1990). Police crackdowns: Initial and residual deterrence. Crime and Justice, 12, 1–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slemrod, J., Blumenthal, M., & Christian, C. (2001). Taxpayer response to an increased probability of audit: Evidence from a controlled experiment in Minnesota. Journal of Public Economics, 79(3), 455–483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Der Weele, J. (2012). Beyond the state of nature: Introducing social interactions in the economic model of crime. Review of Law and Economics, 8(2), 401–432.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Zhixin Dai.

Additional information

We are extremely grateful to P. Jacquesson, R. Sauter and F. German from Keolis and to D. Aubaret and L. Clerc-Gagnoux from Kisio for enabling the realization of this experiment with passengers of the public transportation company in Lyon. We wish to thank an anonymous reviewer and seminar and conference participants in Paris (6th Annual Meeting of ASFEE), Heidelberg (ESA European Meeting), Lyon (6th Annual SEBA-GATE Workshop), and Norwich (CREED-CeDEx-CBESS Meeting). We are very grateful to ASFEE for awarding its Best Paper Award 2015 to this article. We also thank Q. Thevenet for programming the experiment and S. Belhadj, L. Charroin, and A. Solda for research assistance. This research has been supported by a grant from Keolis, a grant of the French National Research Agency (ANR, FELIS Grant, ANR-14-CE28-0010-01), and funding from the European Commission (Marie Skłodowska-Curie Individual Fellowship, 661645-IDEA-MSCA-IF-EF-ST). It was performed within the framework of the LABEX CORTEX (ANR-11-LABX-0042) of Université de Lyon, within the program Investissements d’Avenir (ANR-11-IDEX-007) operated by the French National Research Agency (ANR).

Appendix

Appendix

See Figs. 4 and 5.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Lab in the field

Fig. 5
figure 5

Decision-making screen shot

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Dai, Z., Galeotti, F. & Villeval, M.C. The efficiency of crackdowns: a lab-in-the-field experiment in public transportations. Theory Decis 82, 249–271 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11238-016-9561-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11238-016-9561-0

Keywords

JEL Classification

Navigation