Skip to main content
Log in

Getting it: A predictive processing approach to irony comprehension

  • Published:
Synthese Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

On many occasions, irony is used to communicate emotions, to criticise or to tease other people. Irony comprehension consists in identifying an utterance as ironical and detecting its implied meaning. Existing research has investigated irony comprehension as a pragma-linguistic phenomenon, which has led to several theoretical accounts and interesting empirical results. However, given that irony comprehension is situated in a social context and has the purpose to communicate the mental states of the speaker/writer indirectly, it is reasonable to assume that social cognition and emotional processes play an important role. Until very recently, this has been largely overlooked by research in the field. Furthermore, an overarching framework that can integrate theoretical insights and empirical data on the component processes of irony comprehension is still lacking. The purpose of this paper is to help close this gap. The positive proposal is that the predictive processing framework provides the theoretical resources and conceptual tools to describe relevant aspects of irony comprehension. According to predictive processing, perception, action, cognition, and emotion can be described as a continuous attempt to minimise prediction error. Irony comprehension, I will show, can be depicted as a special case of prediction error minimisation. The neuro-functional mechanism postulated by predictive processing is apt to account for the realisation of the pragma-linguistic, social, and emotional processes that jointly give rise to irony comprehension. The emerging perspective can elucidate why and how people comprehend ironical utterances.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. By talking about the pragma-linguistic component, I would like to emphasise two points: first, pragmatics is an important part of language comprehension. Second, lexical, semantic, and syntactical sub-processes also play an important role in irony comprehension. However, since irony comprehension shares these sub-processes with other types of language processing, I will focus on the pragmatic peculiarities of irony comprehension. In what follows, I will use pragma-linguistic and pragmatic interchangeably to label this component.

  2. Giora’s (1997, 1999) notion of salience is non-technical and clearly distinct from the notion of salience that is frequently used in empirical research on visual attention (see, e.g., Carrasco 2011; Krüger et al. 2017).

  3. Another theoretical account of irony comprehension that is sometimes mentioned (e.g., by Kaakinen et al. 2014) is Pexman’s (2008) constraint satisfaction model. The key idea of this model is that irony comprehension is a function of the satisfaction of constraints on linguistic communication. However, given that the relevant constraints remain largely underdetermined and that it remains unclear how irony comprehension proceeds according to this model, I will not further consider this account.

  4. In the study by Nieuwland and van Berkum (2005), a discourse context is created by introducing a speaker, an interlocutor (e.g., a tourist), and an inanimate object (e.g., a suitcase). In the continuation of the discourse, the interlocutor (e.g., the tourist) is replaced by the inanimate object (e.g., the suitcase), such that the speaker addresses that object rather than another human being. This can give rise to a semantic illusion.

  5. According to Cornejo et al. (2007), the analytic strategy is “characterized by the search and detection of logical semantic incongruities” (p. 422). By contrast, the holistic strategy is “characterized by the search for an expression’s sense in a general context” (ibid.). The participants are asked to categorise each target sentence as either coherent or incoherent. They were instructed to use the analytic strategy by telling them that “it is important you think of the meaning of the sentence and if it is congruent or not with the story” (ibid., 416; italics in original). By contrast, the holistic strategy is induced by asking the participants “to consider if the sentence would make sense in real life, that is, if you can understand what the character means by it” (ibid.; italics in original).

  6. Experiment 1 reported by Filik et al. (2014) will be reviewed below in Sect. 2.2.2.

  7. An example of a familiar ironical target word is ‘tactful’ and an example of an unfamiliar ironical target word is ‘meticulous’ (see Filik et al. 2014, Table 1).

  8. Another notable exception is the fMRI study reported by Akimoto et al. (2014), which uses a presentation format that integrates pictures and written sentences.

  9. For a systematic exploration of the differences between oral and typographic/chirographic communication (see Fabry 2018b).

  10. Activations in the temporo-parietal junction are also frequently associated with perspective taking (Aichhorn et al. 2006), the detection of biological motion (Grossman et al. 2000), and action observation (Blakemore and Decety 2001).

  11. It is an interesting research question in its own right how we should characterise the relationship between irony and humour. See Dynel (2018) for a very elucidating discussion on this question. For current purposes, it is sufficient to assume that at least some instances of ironical utterances are humorous.

  12. The debate concerns the question whether the left vOT area is selectively recruited for visual word recognition only (Dehaene 2010), or whether it still makes important neurofunctional contributions to perceptual and cognitive processes in other domains (Price and Devlin 2003, 2004; Vogel et al. 2012, 2013, 2014). See Fabry (2018a) for a discussion on this issue.

  13. For a preliminary model of irony comprehension in a Bayesian framework, see Kao and Goodman (2015).

  14. I am grateful to an anonymous reviewer for drawing my attention to the commensurability of my interpretation of the word frequency and word predictability effects and the simulations reported by Friston et al. (2018).

  15. I am grateful to an anonymous reviewer for pressing me on this point.

  16. For simulations of epistemic insight directed by epistemic value within the PP framework, see Friston et al. (2017).

  17. The present account of emotional valence can be equally applied to instances of negatively valenced (epistemic) emotions. Negative valence would be associated with a prediction error minimisation rate that is slower (and less efficient) than expected. This could be the case if cognitive agents realise that they have failed to understand a certain utterance, for example. For current purposes, however, I am only interested in providing a preliminary account of positively valenced epistemic emotions (i.e., mirth) within the predictive processing framework.

References

  • Aichhorn, M., Perner, J., Kronbichler, M., Staffen, W., & Ladurner, G. (2006). Do visual perspective tasks need theory of mind? Neuroimage, 30(3), 1059–1068.

    Google Scholar 

  • Akimoto, Y., Sugiura, M., Yomogida, Y., Miyauchi, C. M., Miyazawa, S., & Kawashima, R. (2014). Irony comprehension: Social conceptual knowledge and emotional response. Human Brain Mapping, 35(4), 1167–1178. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allen, M., Frank, D., Schwarzkopf, D. S., Fardo, F., Winston, J. S., Hauser, T. U., et al. (2016). Unexpected arousal modulates the influence of sensory noise on confidence. ELife, 5, 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, M., & Friston, K. J. (2016). From cognitivism to autopoiesis: Towards a computational framework for the embodied mind. Synthese, 2016, 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, M., & Friston, K. J. (2018). From cognitivism to autopoiesis: Towards a computational framework for the embodied mind. Synthese, 195(6), 2459–2482. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-016-1288-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, J., Rayner, K., & Clifton, C. (2005). Eye movements of highly skilled and average readers: Differential effects of frequency and predictability. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 58(6), 1065–1086. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980443000476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bambini, V., Bertini, C., Schaeken, W., Stella, A., & Di Russo, F. (2016). Disentangling metaphor from context: An ERP study. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F. (2017). The theory of constructed emotion: An active inference account of interoception and categorization. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(1), 1–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F., & Simmons, W. K. (2015). Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(7), 419–429.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bedo, N., Ribary, U., Ward, L. M., & Valdes-Sosa, P. A. (2014). Fast dynamics of cortical functional and effective connectivity during word reading. PLoS ONE. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088940.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore, S.-J., & Decety, J. (2001). From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(8), 561–567.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bosco, F. M., Parola, A., Valentini, M. C., & Morese, R. (2017). Neural correlates underlying the comprehension of deceitful and ironic communicative intentions. Cortex, 94, 73–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brouwer, H., Fitz, H., & Hoeks, J. (2012). Getting real about semantic illusions: Rethinking the functional role of the P600 in language comprehension. Brain Research, 1446, 127–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, H., Adams, R. A., Parees, I., Edwards, M., & Friston, K. (2013). Active inference, sensory attenuation and illusions. Cognitive Processing, 14(4), 411–427.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brüne, M., & Bodenstein, L. (2005). Proverb comprehension reconsidered—‘theory of mind’and the pragmatic use of language in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 75(2–3), 233–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, G. A. (2012). Is verbal irony special? Language and Linguistics Compass, 6(11), 673–685.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burkhardt, P. (2007). The P600 reflects cost of new information in discourse memory. NeuroReport, 18(17), 1851–1854.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carrasco, M. (2011). Visual attention: The past 25 years. Vision Research, 51(13), 1484–1525. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2011.04.012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carreiras, M., Armstrong, B. C., Perea, M., & Frost, R. (2014). The what, when, where, and how of visual word recognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(2), 90–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.11.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(03), 181–204. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12000477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2015). Embodied prediction. In T. Metzinger & J. M. Windt (Eds.), Open MIND (pp. 1–21). Frankfurt am Main: MIND Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2016). Surfing uncertainty: Prediction, action, and the embodied mind. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2017). Busting out: Predictive brains, embodied minds, and the puzzle of the evidentiary veil. Noûs, 51(4), 727–753. https://doi.org/10.1111/nous.12140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Specialization within the ventral stream: The case for the visual word form area. NeuroImage, 22(1), 466–476. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.049.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Constable, R. T., Pugh, K. R., Berroya, E., Mencl, W. E., Westerveld, M., Ni, W., et al. (2004). Sentence complexity and input modality effects in sentence comprehension: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 22(1), 11–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cornejo, C., Simonetti, F., Aldunate, N., Ibáñez, A., López, V., & Melloni, L. (2007). Electrophysiological evidence of different interpretative strategies in irony comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36(6), 411–430.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Bruin, L., & Strijbos, D. (2015). Direct social perception, mindreading and Bayesian predictive coding. Consciousness and Cgnition, 36, 565–570.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Jaegher, H., & Di Paolo, E. (2007). Participatory sense-making: An enactive approach to social cognition. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6(4), 485–507.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (2010). Reading in the brain: The new science of how we read. New York: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (2011). The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(6), 254–262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drieghe, D., Brysbaert, M., Desmet, T., & Baecke, C. (2004). Word skipping in reading: On the interplay of linguistic and visual factors. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16(1–2), 79–103. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440340000141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dynel, M. (2018). Irony, deception and humour: Seeking the truth about overt and covert untruthfulness. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fabry, R. E. (2017). Predictive processing and cognitive development. In T. K. Metzinger & W. Wiese (Eds.), Philosophy and predictive processing (pp. 1–18). Frankfurt am Main: MIND Group. https://doi.org/10.15502/9783958573147.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fabry, R. E. (2018a). Betwixt and between: The enculturated predictive processing approach to cognition. Synthese, 195(6), 2483–2518. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-017-1334-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fabry, R. E. (2018b). Enculturation and narrative practices. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 17(5), 911–937. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-018-9567-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fabry, R. E., & Kukkonen, K. (2019). Reconsidering the mind-wandering reader: Predictive processing, probability designs, and enculturation. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02648.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farmer, T. A., Brown, M., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2013). Prediction, explanation, and the role of generative models in language processing. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(03), 211–212. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12002312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, H., & Friston, K. J. (2010). Attention, uncertainty, and free-energy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2010.00215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Filik, R., Brightman, E., Gathercole, C., & Leuthold, H. (2017). The emotional impact of verbal irony: Eye-tracking evidence for a two-stage process. Journal of Memory and Language, 93, 193–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2016.09.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Filik, R., Leuthold, H., Wallington, K., & Page, J. (2014). Testing theories of irony processing using eye-tracking and ERPs. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(3), 811–828.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fine, A. B., & Florian Jaeger, T. (2013). Evidence for Implicit Learning in Syntactic Comprehension. Cognitive Science, 37(3), 578–591. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fine, A. B., Jaeger, T. F., Farmer, T. A., Qian, T., & Paterson, K. (2013). Rapid Expectation Adaptation during Syntactic Comprehension. PLoS ONE, 8(10), e77661. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0077661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, S. L., Otten, L. J., Galli, G., & Vigliocco, G. (2015). The ERP response to the amount of information conveyed by words in sentences. Brain and Language, 140, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2014.10.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franke, M., & Jäger, G. (2016). Probabilistic pragmatics, or why Bayes’ rule is probably important for pragmatics. Zeitschrift Für Sprachwissenschaft, 35(1), 3–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friston, K. (2005). A theory of cortical responses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 360(1456), 815–836. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2005.1622.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friston, Karl. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(2), 127–138. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2787.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friston, Karl, Adams, R. A., Perrinet, L., & Breakspear, M. (2012). Perceptions as Hypotheses: Saccades as Experiments. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friston, K. J., Lin, M., Frith, C. D., Pezzulo, G., Hobson, J. A., & Ondobaka, S. (2017). Active inference, curiosity and insight. Neural Computation, 29(10), 2633–2683.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friston, Karl, Rigoli, F., Ognibene, D., Mathys, C., Fitzgerald, T., & Pezzulo, G. (2015). Active inference and epistemic value. Cognitive Neuroscience, 6(4), 187–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friston, K. J., Rosch, R., Parr, T., Price, C., & Bowman, H. (2018). Deep temporal models and active inference. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 90, 486–501.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (2003). Autism. Explaining the enigma (2nd ed.). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (1999). Interacting minds—A biological basis. Science, 286(5445), 1692–1695.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 459–473.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2008). Direct perception in the intersubjective context. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(2), 535–543.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M. A., & Pripas-Kapit, S. R. (2012). Who’s missing the point? A commentary on claims that autistic persons have a specific deficit in figurative language comprehension. Metaphor and Symbol, 27(1), 93–105. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2012.656255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gervais, M., & Wilson, D. S. (2005). The evolution and functions of laughter and humor: A synthetic approach. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 80(4), 395–430.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs, R. W., Jr. (2002). A new look at literal meaning in understanding what is said and implicated. Journal of Pragmatics, 34(4), 457–486.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs, R. W., Bryant, G. A., & Colston, H. L. (2014). Where is the humor in verbal irony? Humor, 27(4), 575–595.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs, R. W., & Colston, H. L. (2006). Figurative language. In M. Traxler & M. A. Gernsbacher (Eds.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (2nd ed., pp. 835–861). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs, R. W., Samermit, P., & Karzmark, C. R. (2018). Humor, irony, and the body. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 16(1), 72–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giora, R. (1997). Understanding figurative and literal language: The graded salience hypothesis. Cognitive Linguistics, 8(3), 183–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giora, R. (1999). On the priority of salient meanings: Studies of literal and figurative language. Journal of Pragmatics, 31(7), 919–929.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, N. D., & Frank, M. C. (2016). Pragmatic language interpretation as probabilistic inference. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(11), 818–829.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. L. Morgan (Eds.), Speech acts: Syntax and semantics (pp. 41–58). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G., et al. (2000). Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 711–720.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagoort, P., Hald, L., Bastiaansen, M., & Petersson, K. M. (2004). Integration of word meaning and world knowledge in language comprehension. Science, 304(5669), 438–441.

    Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. G. E. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48(2), 101–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. (2016). Born pupils? Natural pedagogy and cultural pedagogy. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(2), 280–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. (2018). Cognitive gadgets: The cultural evolution of thinking. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. M., & Frith, C. D. (2014). The cultural evolution of mind reading. Science, 344(6190), 12430911–12430916.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hohwy, J. (2011). Phenomenal variability and introspective reliability. Mind and Language, 26(3), 261–286. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2011.01418.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hohwy, J. (2012). Attention and conscious perception in the hypothesis testing brain. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00096.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hohwy, J. (2013). The predictive mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hohwy, J. (2015). The neural organ explains the mind. In T. Metzinger & J. M. Windt (Eds.), Open MIND (pp. 1–22). Frankfurt am Main: MIND Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hohwy, J., & Palmer, C. (2014). Social cognition as causal inference: Implications for common knowledge and autism. In M. Gallotti & J. Michael (Eds.), Perspectives on social ontology and social cognition (pp. 167–189). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurley, M. M., Dennett, D. C., & Adams, R. B. (2011). Inside jokes: Using humor to reverse-engineer the mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jobard, G., Vigneau, M., Mazoyer, B., & Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2007). Impact of modality and linguistic complexity during reading and listening tasks. NeuroImage, 34(2), 784–800. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.067.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joffily, M., & Coricelli, G. (2013). Emotional valence and the free-energy principle. PLoS Computational Biology, 9(6), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003094.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juhasz, B. J., & Rayner, K. (2003). Investigating the Effects of a Set of Intercorrelated Variables on Eye Fixation Durations in Reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(6), 1312–1318. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.29.6.1312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaakinen, J. K., Olkoniemi, H., Kinnari, T., & Hyönä, J. (2014). Processing of written irony: An eye movement study. Discourse Processes, 51(4), 287–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kao, J. T., & Goodman, N. D. (2015). Let’s talk (ironically) about the weather: Modeling verbal irony. In D. C. Noelle, R. Dale, A. S. Warlaumont, J. Yoshimi, T. Matlock, C. D. Jennings, & P. P. Maglio (Eds.), Proceedings of the 37th annual meeting of the cognitive science society (pp. 1051–1056). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller, T. A., Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (2001). The Neural Bases of Sentence Comprehension: A fMRI Examination of Syntactic and Lexical Processing. Cerebral Cortex, 11(3), 223–237. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/11.3.223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kherif, F., Josse, G., & Price, C. J. (2011). Automatic top-down processing explains common left occipito-temporal responses to visual words and objects. Cerebral Cortex, 21(1), 103–114. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq063.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirchhoff, M. (2018). Predictive brains and embodied, enactive cognition: An introduction to the special issue. Synthese, 195(6), 2355–2366. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-017-1534-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kiverstein, J., Miller, M., & Rietveld, E. (2017). The feeling of grip: Novelty, error dynamics, and the predictive brain. Synthese. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-017-1583-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleckner, I. R., Zhang, J., Touroutoglou, A., Chanes, L., Xia, C., Simmons, W. K., et al. (2017). Evidence for a large-scale brain system supporting allostasis and interoception in humans. Nature Human Behaviour, 1, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0069.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kliegl, R., Grabner, E., Rolfs, M., & Engbert, R. (2004). Length, frequency, and predictability effects of words on eye movements in reading. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16(1–2), 262–284. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440340000213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krüger, A., Tünnermann, J., & Scharlau, I. (2017). Measuring and modeling salience with the theory of visual attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79(6), 1593–1614. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-017-1325-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuperberg, G. R. (2007). Neural mechanisms of language comprehension: Challenges to syntax. Brain Research, 1146, 23–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuperberg, G. R., & Jaeger, T. F. (2016). What do we mean by prediction in language comprehension? Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(1), 32–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621–647.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ledoux, K., Traxler, M. J., & Swaab, T. Y. (2007). Syntactic Priming in Comprehension: Evidence From Event-Related Potentials. Psychological Science, 18(2), 135–143. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01863.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leggitt, J. S., & Gibbs, R. W. (2000). Emotional reactions to verbal irony. Discourse Processes, 29(1), 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCandliss, B. D., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2003). The visual word form area: Expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(7), 293–299. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00134-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R. (2015). What? Now: Predictive coding and enculturation: A reply to Regina E. Fabry. In T. Metzinger & J. M. Windt (Eds.), Open MIND (pp. 1–8). Frankfurt am Main: MIND Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J. (2011a). Interactionism and mindreading. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 2(3), 559.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J. (2011b). Shared emotions and joint action. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 2(2), 355–373. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-011-0055-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J. (2016). What are shared emotions (for)? Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1–3. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michael, E. B., Keller, T. A., Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (2001). fMRI investigation of sentence comprehension by eye and by ear: Modality fingerprints on cognitive processes. Human Brain Mapping, 13(4), 239–252. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.1036.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, M., & Clark, A. (2018). Happily entangled: Prediction, Pmotion, and the embodied mind. Synthese, 195(6), 2559–2575. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-017-1399-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nieuwland, M. S., & Van Berkum, J. J. A. (2005). Testing the limits of the semantic illusion phenomenon: ERPs reveal temporary semantic change deafness in discourse comprehension. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(3), 691–701.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nieuwland, M. S., & Van Berkum, J. J. A. (2006). When peanuts fall in love: N400 evidence for the power of discourse. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(7), 1098–1111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noppeney, U., & Price, C. J. (2004). An fMRI study of syntactic adaptation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(4), 702–713. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892904323057399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Obert, A., Gierski, F., Calmus, A., Flucher, A., Portefaix, C., Pierot, L., et al. (2016). Neural correlates of contrast and humor: Processing common features of verbal irony. PLoS ONE, 11(11), 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ondobaka, S., Kilner, J., & Friston, K. (2017). The role of interoceptive inference in theory of mind. Brain and Cognition, 112, 64–68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2015.08.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Otten, M., Seth, A. K., & Pinto, Y. (2017). A social Bayesian brain: How social knowledge can shape visual perception. Brain and Cognition, 112, 69–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, C. J., Seth, A. K., & Hohwy, J. (2015). The felt presence of other minds: Predictive processing, counterfactual predictions, and mentalising in autism. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 376–389. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.04.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pexman, P. M. (2008). It’s fascinating research: The cognition of verbal irony. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(4), 286–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pezzulo, G., Cartoni, E., Rigoli, F., Pio-Lopez, L., & Friston, K. (2016). Active Inference, epistemic value, and vicarious trial and error. Learning & Memory, 23(7), 322–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pickup, G. J., & Frith, C. D. (2001). Theory of mind impairments in schizophrenia: Symptomatology, severity and specificity. Psychological Medicine, 31(2), 207–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Price, C. J., & Devlin, J. T. (2003). The myth of the visual word form area. NeuroImage, 19(3), 473–481. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00084-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Price, C. J., & Devlin, J. T. (2004). The pro and cons of labelling a left occipitotemporal region “the visual word form area”. NeuroImage, 22(1), 477–479.

    Google Scholar 

  • Price, C. J., & Devlin, J. T. (2011). The interactive account of ventral occipitotemporal contributions to reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(6), 246–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quattrocki, E., & Friston, K. (2014). Autism, oxytocin and interoception. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 47, 410–430. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.09.012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372–422. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (2009). Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(8), 1457–1506. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210902816461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., Slattery, T. J., Drieghe, D., & Liversedge, S. P. (2011). Eye movements and word skipping during reading: Effects of word length and predictability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(2), 514–528. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020990.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Regel, S., Coulson, S., & Gunter, T. C. (2010). The communicative style of a speaker can affect language comprehension: ERP evidence from the comprehension of irony. Brain Research, 1311, 121–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Regel, S., Gunter, T. C., & Friederici, A. D. (2011). Isn’t it ironic? An electrophysiological exploration of figurative language processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(2), 277–293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roepstorff, A. (2013). Interactively human: Sharing time, constructing materiality. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(03), 224–225. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12002427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schurz, M., Kronbichler, M., Crone, J., Richlan, F., Klackl, J., & Wimmer, H. (2014). Top-down and bottom-up influences on the left ventral occipito-temporal cortex during visual word recognition: An analysis of effective connectivity. Human Brain Mapping, 35(4), 1668–1680. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. R. (1979). Expression and meaning: Studies in the theory of speech acts. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seth, A. K. (2013). Interoceptive inference, emotion, and the embodied self. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(11), 565–573. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seth, A. K. (2015). The cybernetic Bayesian brain: From interoceptive inference to sensorimotor contingencies. In T. Metzinger & J. M. Windt (Eds.), Open MIND (pp. 1–24). Frankfurt am Main: MIND Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seth, A. K., & Friston, K. J. (2016). Active interoceptive inference and the emotional brain. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B, 371(1708), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shibata, M., Toyomura, A., Itoh, H., & Abe, J. (2010). Neural substrates of irony comprehension: A functional MRI study. Brain Research, 1308, 114–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snijders, T. M., Vosse, T., Kempen, G., van Berkum, J. J. A., Petersson, K. M., & Hagoort, P. (2009). Retrieval and Unification of Syntactic Structure in Sentence Comprehension: An fMRI Study Using Word-Category Ambiguity. Cerebral Cortex, 19(7), 1493–1503. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1995). Relevance: Communication and cognition (2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spotorno, N., Cheylus, A., Van Der Henst, J.-B., & Noveck, I. A. (2013). What’s behind a P600? Integration operations during irony processing. PLoS ONE, 8(6), 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spotorno, N., Koun, E., Prado, J., Van Der Henst, J.-B., & Noveck, I. A. (2012). Neural evidence that utterance-processing entails mentalizing: The case of irony. NeuroImage, 63(1), 25–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Twomey, T., Kawabata Duncan, K. J., Price, C. J., & Devlin, J. T. (2011). Top-down modulation of ventral occipito-temporal responses during visual word recognition. NeuroImage, 55(3), 1242–1251. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van de Cruys, S. (2017). Affective value in the predictive mind. In T. K. Metzinger & W. Wiese (Eds.), Philosophy and predictive processing (pp. 1–21). Frankfurt am Main: MIND Group. https://doi.org/10.15502/9783958573253.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Van Petten, C., & Luka, B. J. (2012). Prediction during language comprehension: Benefits, costs, and ERP components. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 83(2), 176–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varga, E., Simon, M., Tényi, T., Schnell, Z., Hajnal, A., Orsi, G., et al. (2013). Irony comprehension and context processing in schizophrenia during remission–A functional MRI study. Brain and Language, 126(3), 231–242. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2013.05.017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, A. C., Church, J. A., Power, J. D., Miezin, F. M., Petersen, S. E., & Schlaggar, B. L. (2013). Functional network architecture of reading-related regions across development. Brain and Language, 125(2), 231–243. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2012.12.016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, A. C., Petersen, S. E., & Schlaggar, B. L. (2012). The left occipitotemporal cortex does not show preferential activity for words. Cerebral Cortex, 22(12), 2715–2732.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, A. C., Petersen, S. E., & Schlaggar, B. L. (2014). The VWFA: It’s not just for words anymore. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00088.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vrticka, P., Black, J. M., & Reiss, A. L. (2013). The neural basis of humour processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(12), 860–868.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warren, C., & McGraw, A. P. (2015). Opinion: What makes things humorous. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(23), 7105–7106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werning, M., & Cosentino, E. (2017). The interaction of Bayesian pragmatics and lexical semantics in linguistic interpretation: Using event-related potentials to investigate hearers’ probabilistic predictions. In Proceedings of the 39th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 3504–3509).

  • White, S., Rayner, K., & Liversedge, S. (2005). The influence of parafoveal word length and contextual constraint on fixation durations and word skipping in reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(3), 466–471. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193789.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I am greatly indebted to Markus Pantsar and two anonymous reviewers for their constructive feedback on previous versions of this paper. I would also like to thank the audience of the second workshop of the ERC-funded project X-Spect: Embodied prediction and the construction of conscious experience on Predictive processing: State of the art (and just beyond) (University of Edinburgh, November 2018), as well as Peter Brössel, Insa Lawler, Matthias Unterhuber, and Markus Werning for insightful comments on earlier versions of this work.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Regina E. Fabry.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Fabry, R.E. Getting it: A predictive processing approach to irony comprehension. Synthese 198, 6455–6489 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-019-02470-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-019-02470-9

Keywords

Navigation