Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Zooming in on Life Events: Is Hedonic Adaptation Sensitive to the Temporal Distance from the Event?

  • Published:
Social Indicators Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper analyzed the effect of major positive and negative life events (marriage, divorce, birth of child, widowhood, and unemployment) on life satisfaction. For the first time, this study estimated the effects of life events not with a precision of 12 months but of 3 months. Specifically, two questions were addressed: (1) Does the precision of the temporal localization of the event (i.e., 12 or 3 months) affect the observed trajectories of life satisfaction, and (2) is the precision of the temporal localization more important for negative life events? As expected, results showed that the precision of temporal localization allows a clearer view on hedonic adaptation, in particular following negative life events.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. To our knowledge, only Frijters et al. (2011) apply quarterly timing of life events to large-scale panel data (HILDA); they do not focus, though, on the comparison of yearly and quarterly measurements.

  2. On the other hand, Zimmermann and Easterlin (2006) apply different models to the same dataset (GSOEP) and report that individuals who remain married for 2 or more years do not go back to their pre-marriage baseline but rather remain at a higher level.

  3. Moreover, continuous exposure to unemployment may evoke sensitization - an increase of the initial reaction rather than adaptation (Frederick and Loewenstein 1999; Luhmann and Eid 2009).

  4. For women.

  5. Given that the data in the cited sources were collected at different decades of the twentieth century, one may also hypothesize that, incidentally, cohort effects may play a role.

  6. I.e. within 2 years following the loss of spouse, SBW increases significantly from one 6-month period to another. This pattern does not apply to divorce. This study takes into account only 2 years after divorce/widowhood; it is plausible that time becomes a predictor if longer time span is considered.

  7. More precisely, for women, anticipation of unemployment, birth of child and layoff lasts for about 1 year, whereas men anticipate divorce for 3 years, marriage and widowhood—for 2 years. In females, there are no lead effects in cases of marriage and birth of child, unemployment is anticipated for about 1 year, widowhood—for 3 years and divorce—for 4 years.

  8. The picture is, however, more complicated. Research on repeated life events reveals, in fact, different patterns of well-being dynamics; in case of repeated unemployment we observe sensitization rather than adaptation, whereas repeated marriages remain as good as the first one, whereas second divorces evokes weaker response than the first one (Luhmann and Eid 2009). Apparently, individual normality intertwines with other factors.

  9. To analyze the anticipatory stage of adaptation process.

  10. The first interview after the event took place.

  11. Since the time span between two interviews is sometimes less than 12 months, it happens than an individual appears in two groups, i.e. once in the group of respondents who will experience an event in 10–12 months, and again in the group of those who will experience it within 3 months after the interview. The numbers of such cases is small: 17 for marriage, 5 for divorce, 13 for unemployment, 15 for birth of child, and 10 for widowhood.

  12. If, for example, the sample were split into two equal halves, the problem of too small cell sized would have been encountered. In fact, even with 2/3 of the whole sample we encounter this problem in two subgroups in the case of unemployment.

  13. Tables 3 and 4 present estimates obtained on the whole sample. The coefficients obtained with split sample provide virtually the same results.

  14. With respect to the relationship between control variables and SWB, our findings, in general, support the exitant literature. Having health problems, being widowed (as compared to being single), being unemployed, having an unemployed partner, and higher number of children affect SWB negatively. Being married (as compared to being single), and higher household income are positively related to SWB.

  15. *** Significant at 0.01; ** Significant at 0.05; * Significant at 0.1.

  16. Even though divorce might be the exit from an unhappy marriage, its short-term consequences, as well as the period of anticipation, are associated with lower life satisfaction.

  17. This brings us to the question whether events as discrete points are good markers of a critical loss or gain: if an event as such is a critical marker of change in SWB, how wide are the time brackets that limit the initial reaction?.

  18. Even though, improvement in SWB can also be observed right after death of the partner; usually, such trajectory is characteristic for cases of long-term care.

  19. Although there are highly predictable cases of caring for a sick partner, as mentioned above, uncertainty regarding the exact timing of the loss still persists.

References

  • Amato, P. R. (2000). The consequences of divorce for adults and children. Journal of Marriage & Family, 62(4), 1269–1287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, S. H. (2008). The short- and long-term effects of government training on subjective well-being. European Sociological Review, 24(4), 451–462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andress, H., & Brackel, M. (2007). Income and life satisfaction after marital disruption in Germany. Journal of Marriage & Family, 69(2), 500–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanchflower, D. G., & Oswald, A. J. (2004). Well-Being over time in Britain and the USA. Journal of Public Economics, 88, 1359–1386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonanno, G. A., Lehman, D. R., Tweed, R. G., Haring, M., Wortman, C. B., Sonnega, J., et al. (2002). Resilience to loss and chronic grief: A prospective study from preloss to 18-months postloss. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(5), 1150–1164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brickman, P., & Campbell, D. T. (1971). Hedonic Relativism and planning the good society. In M. H. Appley (Ed.), Adaptation-level theory (pp. 287–302). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brickman, P., Coates, D., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (1978). Lottery winners and accident victims: Is happiness relative? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 917–927.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burke, C. T., Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2007). Individual differences in adjustment to spousal loss: A nonlinear mixed model analysis. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31(4), 405–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carnelley, K. B., Wortman, C. B., Bolger, N., & Burke, C. T. (2006). The time course of grief reactions to spousal loss: Evidence from a national probability sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 476–492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. E., Diener, E., Georgellis, Y., & Lucas, R. E. (2008). Lags and leads in life satisfaction: A test of the baseline hypothesis. Economic Journal, 118(529), F222–F243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. E., Knabe, A., & Rätzel, S. (2009). Unemployment as a social norm in Germany. Schmollers Jahrbuch, 129(2), 251–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A. (1983). Comparing regression coefficients across subsamples: A study of the statistical test. Sociological Methods & Research, 12(1), 77–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davern, M., Cummins, R. A., & Stokes, M. (2007). Subjective wellbeing as an affective/cognitive construct. Journal of Happiness Studies, 8, 429–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Lucas, R. (1999). Personality and subjective well-being. In D. Kahneman & E. Diener (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 213–229). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Lucas, R. E., & Scollon, C. N. (2006). Beyond the hedonic treadmill—revising the adaptation theory of well-being. American Psychologist, 61(4), 305–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Suh, E., & Oishi, S. (1997). Recent findings on subjective well-being. Indian Journal of Clinical Psychology, 24, 25–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erbes, J. T., & Hedderson, J. J. C. (1984). A longitudinal examination of the separation/divorce process. Journal of Marriage & Family, 46(4), 937–941.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farnsworth, J., Pett, M. A., & Lund, D. A. (1989). Predictors of loss management and well-being in later life widowhood and divorce. Journal of Family Issues, 10(1), 102–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forste, R., & Heaton, T. B. (2004). The divorce generation: well-being, family attitudes, and socioeconomic consequences of marital disruption. Journal of Divorce & Remarriage, 41(1/2), 95–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frederick, S., & Loewenstein, G. (1999). Hedonic adaptation. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 302–329). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frijters, P., Johnston, D., & Shields, M. A. (2011). Happiness dynamics with quarterly life event data. Scandinavian Journal of Economics, 113(1), 190–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gähler, M. (2006). To divorce is to die a bit: A longitudinal study of marital disruption and psychological distress among Swedish women and men. Family Journal, 14(4), 372–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, C., Eggers, A., & Sukhtankar, S. (2004). Does happiness pay? An exploration based on panel data from Russia. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 55(3), 319–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greve, W., & Staudinger, U. M. (2006). Resilience in later adulthood and old age: Resources and potentials for successful aging. In D. Cicchetti & A. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (2nd ed., pp. 796–840). New York: Wiley.

  • Grob, A., Little, T. D., Wanner, B., & Wearning, A. J. (1996). Adolescents’ well-being and perceived control across 14 sociocultural contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 785–795.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haisken-DeNew, J. P. & Frick, J. R. (Eds.). (2005). Desktop companion to the german socio-economic panel (SOEP).version 8.0. Berlin: DIW (German institute for economic research).

  • Headey, B. (2008). The set-point theory of well-being: Negative results and consequent revisions. Social Indicators Research, 85, 389–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Headey, B., & Wearing, A. (1989). Personality, life events, and subjective well-being: Toward a dynamic equilibrium model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 731–739.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1984). Choices, values and frames. American Psychologist, 39, 341–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitson, G. C., & Morgan, L. A. (1990). The multiple consequences of divorce: A decade review. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 913–924.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R. E. (2005). Time does not heal all wounds. Psychological Science, 16(12), 945–950.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R. E. (2007). Long-term disability is associated with lasting changes in subjective well-being: Evidence from two nationally representative longitudinal studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 717–730.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R. E., Clark, A. E., Georgellis, Y., & Diener, E. (2004). Unemployment alters the set point for life satisfaction. Psychological Science, 15(1), 8–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R. E., Georgellis, Y., Clark, A. E., & Diener, E. (2003). Reexamining adaptation and the set point model of happiness: Reactions to changes in marital status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(3), 527–539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luhmann, M., & Eid, M. (2009). Does it really feel the same? Changes in life satisfaction following repeated life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(2), 363–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S. (2011). Hedonic adaptation to positive and negative experiences. In S. Folkman (Ed.), Oxford handbook of stress, health, and coping (pp. 200–224). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundlak, Y. (1978). On the pooling of time series and cross section data. Econometrica, 46, 69–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oishi, S., Diener, E., Choi, D.-W., Kim-Prieto, C., & Choi, I. (2007). The dynamics of daily events and well-being across cultures: When less is more. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(4), 685–698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratnikova, T. A. (2006). Introduction to econometric analysis of panel data. HSE Economic Journal., 2, 267–316.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staudinger, U. M. (2000). Viele Gründe sprechen dagegen und trotzdem fühlen viele Menschen sich wohl: Das Paradox des subjektiven Wohlbefindens [Many reasons speak against it but many people are happy: The well-being paradox]. Psychologische Rundschau, 51, 185–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Staudinger, U. M., Marsiske, M., & Baltes, P. B. (1995). Resilience and reserve capacity in later adulthood: Potentials and limits of development across the life span. In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (Vol. 2: Risk, disorder, and adaptation; pp. 801–847). New York: Wiley.

  • Stutzer, A., & Frey, B. S. (2006). Does marriage make people happy, or do happy people get married? Journal of Socio-Economics, 35(2), 326–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suh, E., Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1996). Events and subjective well-being: Only recent events matter. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(5), 1091–1102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vallin, J., & Nizard, A. (1977). La mortalité par état matrimonial. Mariage sélection ou mariage protection. Population (French Edition), 32, 95–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2008). Explaining away: A model of affective adaptation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(5), 370–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmermann, A. C., & Easterlin, R. A. (2006). Happily ever after? Cohabitation, marriage, divorce, and happiness in Germany. Population & Development Review, 32(3), 511–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Preparation of this manuscript was carried out as a part of the doctoral work of the first author within the graduate program of the Bremen International Graduate School of Social Sciences (BIGSSS; funded by the German Research Foundation DFG). We thank Reinhard Schunk and Liuben Siarov for their comments on the analytic strategy of the study. We also wish to acknowledge helpful comments received during the review process.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ekaterina A. Uglanova.

Appendices

Appendix 1

See Table 6.

Table 6 The distribution of life satisfaction ratings in the SOEP subsample of western Germans, 1984–2007

Appendix 2

See Table 7.

Table 7 Adaptation to life events

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Uglanova, E.A., Staudinger, U.M. Zooming in on Life Events: Is Hedonic Adaptation Sensitive to the Temporal Distance from the Event?. Soc Indic Res 111, 265–286 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-012-0004-1

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-012-0004-1

Keywords

Navigation