Skip to main content
Log in

What Can Be Learned From a Laboratory Model of Conceptual Change? Descriptive Findings and Methodological Issues

  • Published:
Science & Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The problem of how people process novel and unexpected information—deep learning (Ohlsson in Deep learning: how the mind overrides experience. Cambridge University Press, New York, 2011)—is central to several fields of research, including creativity, belief revision, and conceptual change. Researchers have not converged on a single theory for conceptual change, nor has any one theory been decisively falsified. One contributing reason is the difficulty of collecting informative data in this field. We propose that the commonly used methodologies of historical analysis, classroom interventions, and developmental studies, although indispensible, can be supplemented with studies of laboratory models of conceptual change. We introduce re-categorization, an experimental paradigm in which learners transition from one definition of a categorical concept to another, incompatible definition of the same concept, a simple form of conceptual change. We describe a re-categorization experiment, report some descriptive findings pertaining to the effects of category complexity, the temporal unfolding of learning, and the nature of the learner’s final knowledge state. We end with a brief discussion of ways in which the re-categorization model can be improved.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. See e.g., Carey (2009), Chi (2005, 2008), Nersessian (2008), Ohlsson (2009), Sinatra and Pintrich (2003), and Vosniadou et al. (2007) for examples of theories of conceptual change.

  2. For overviews of categorization research, see e.g., Ashby and Maddox (2005), Cohen and Lefebvre (2005), Margolis and Laurence (1999), Markman and Ross (2003) and Ross et al. (2008). For examples of recent empirical studies, see, e.g., Craig and Lewandowsky (2012) and Lafond et al. (2009).

  3. For convenience, we will use the term “definition” to refer to the mental representation of the meaning of a concept. We do not mean to imply that such a representation consists of a list of necessary and sufficient conditions for category membership.

References

  • Ashby, F. G., & Maddox, W. T. (2005). Human category learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 149–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H. (2005). Commonsense conceptions of emergent processes: Why some misconceptions are robust. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 14, 161–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H. (2008). Three types of conceptual change: Belief revision, mental model transformation, and categorical shift. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), Handbook of research on conceptual change (pp. 61–82). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, H., & Lefebvre, C. (Eds.). (2005). Handbook of categorization in cognitive science. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Comins, N. F. (2001). Heavenly errors: Misconceptions about the real nature of the universe. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosejo, D. G., Oesterreich, J., & Ohlsson, S. (2009). Re-categorization: Restructuring in categorization. In N. A. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craig, S., & Lewandowsky, S. (2012). Whichever way you choose to categorize, working memory helps you learn. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(3), 439–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiSessa, A. A. (1993). Toward and epistemology of physics. Cognition and Instruction, 10, 105–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiSessa, A. A., Gillespie, N. M., & Esterly, J. B. (2004). Coherence versus fragmentation in the development of the concept of force. Cognitive Science, 28, 843–890.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duit, R., & Treagust, D. F. (2003). Conceptual change: A powerful framework for improving science teaching and learning. International Journal of Science Education, 25(6), 671–688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, K. (2001). The analogical paradox: Why analogy is so easy in naturalistic settings, yet so difficult in the psychological laboratory. In D. Gentner, K. Holyoak, & B. Kokinov (Eds.), The analogical mind: Perspectives from cognitive science (pp. 313–334). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, K., & Blanchette, I. (2001). The in vivo/in vitro approach to cognition: The case of analogy. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 334–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flynn, E., & Siegler, R. (2007). Measuring change: Current trends and future directions in microgenetic research. Infant and Child Development, 16, 135–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1997). Words, thoughts, and theories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, H. E. (1974). Darwin on man: A psychological study of scientific creativity. London, UK: Wildwood House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitcher, P. (1993). The advancement of science. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd ed.). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lafond, D., Lacouture, Y., & Cohen, A. L. (2009). Decision-tree models of categorization response times, choice proportions, and typicality judgments. Psychological Review, 116(4), 833–855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K., & Lippitt, R. (1938). An experimental approach to the study of autocracy and democracy: A preliminary note. Sociometry, 1, 292–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Limón, M. (2001). On the cognitive conflict as an instructional strategy for conceptual change: a critical appraisal. Learning and Instruction, 11, 357–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Margolis, E., & Laurence, S. (Eds.). (1999). Concepts: Core readings. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, A. B., & Ross, B. H. (2003). Category use and category learning. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 592–613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N. J. (2008). Creating scientific concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ohlsson, S. (2009). Resubsumption: A possible mechanism for conceptual change and belief revision. Educational Psychologist, 44, 20–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ohlsson, S. (2011). Deep learning: How the mind overrides experience. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, S., & Tomie, A. (2007). Children’s performance on and understanding of the balance scale problem: The effects of parental support. Infant and Child Development, 16, 95–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Popper, K. (1959/1972). The logic of scientific discovery (translation by author, revised version). London, UK: Hutchinson.

  • Posner, G. J., Strike, K. A., Hewson, P. W., & Gertzog, W. A. (1982). Accommodation of a scientific conception: Toward a theory of conceptual change. Science Education, 66, 211–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rakison, D. H., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2001). Developmental origin of the animate–inanimate distinction. Psychological Bulletin, 127(2), 209–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, B. H., Taylor, E. G., Middleton, E. L., & Nokes, T. J. (2008). Concept and category learning in humans. Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference, 2, 535–556.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B., Perret-Clermont, A.-N., Trognon, A., & Marro, P. (2008). Emergent learning in successive activities. Pragmatics & Cognition, 16, 57–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shipstone, D. M. (1984). A study of children’s understanding of electricity in simple DC circuits. European Journal of Science Education, 6, 185–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinatra, G. M., & Pintrich, P. R. (Eds.). (2003). Intentional conceptual change. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thagard, P. (1992). Conceptual revolutions. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., Baltas, A., & Vamvakoussi, X. (Eds.). (2007). Reframing the conceptual change approach to learning and instruction. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1992). Mental models of the earth: A study of conceptual change in childhood. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 535–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Voutsina, C. (2012). Procedural and conceptual changes in young children’s problem solving. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 79, 193–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The work reported in this article was supported, in part, by Award N00014-09-1-0125 from the Office of Naval Research (ONR), US Navy, to the first author. No endorsement should be inferred. We thank Bettina Chow for assistance in the development of the experimental stimuli and Justin Oesterreich for programming the first re-categorization experiment in the E-Prime laboratory software.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stellan Ohlsson.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Ohlsson, S., Cosejo, D.G. What Can Be Learned From a Laboratory Model of Conceptual Change? Descriptive Findings and Methodological Issues. Sci & Educ 23, 1485–1504 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-013-9658-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-013-9658-6

Keywords

Navigation