Skip to main content
Log in

Predicted risk perception and risk-taking behavior: The case of impaired driving

  • Published:
Journal of Risk and Uncertainty Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Research suggests an association between risk perception and risk-taking behavior in a variety of contexts. There is empirical evidence that perceived risk is generally biased and that perception of risk influences behavior. Perception of risk can be endogenous. It is therefore more appropriate to instrument risk perception. This article studies the perception of the risks associated with impaired driving and the relation between predicted risk perception and driving behavior. We survey a sample of license-holders, half of whom are drivers with a past conviction for impaired driving, the other half or control group without such conviction. Predicted perceptual biases are shown to influence actual driving behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For an assessment, see Benson and Rasmussen (1999), Young and Likens (2000), and Eisenberg (2003).

  2. See Arrow (1982) for a review of the first generation of studies and a discussion of the relation between learning processes and the rationality of behaviors.

  3. In the Phelps study, the perceived relative risk of DUI versus driving while sober ranges from 1.47 to 7.43, depending on the number of drinks considered. In Levitt and Porter (see their Table 7 for drivers under 25 years old), the estimated relative risk for drivers in that age group is between 3.9 and 4.7.

  4. See also the recent study of Lundborg (2007).

  5. We assume that individuals truthfully answer when evaluating different risks which does not necessarily eliminate endogenous problems and perception biases. Indeed, we do not see why there should be any personal advantage to lie when responding to questions about risk perception.

  6. Polinsky and Shavell (2000) present a general review of policies of dissuasion; Zaal (1999) gives a summary of the literature on measures encouraging compliance with the Safety Code. See also Paternoster (1987).

  7. Detailed analysis of the survey procedure is presented in Appendix 1 on the web site.

  8. Details are given in Appendix 1 on the web site.

  9. For details, see Table 17 in Appendix 2 on the web site.

  10. For details, see Table 18 in Appendix 2 on the web site.

  11. We decided to have the same number of potential drivers for all questions. We choose 20,000 in order to have a feasible interval for bodily injury accidents, as discussed later.

  12. The reestimated equations are in Table 1A of Appendix 3.1 on the web site. We did also use all the variables in Table 2 and obtained the same results. They are available from the authors. Which methodology dominates is still an open question in the literature. We did a paired t-test on the difference between the two predictions (with all variables or with only the significant ones). We did not reject the null hypothesis that the difference between the two predictions is 0. Details are available.

  13. Complete regressions are available in Appendix 3.2 on the web site.

  14. We used the estimations presented in Table 2A on the web site.

  15. The predicted perceptions come from Table 3A on the web site.

  16. The estimated perceptions are computed from regressions in Tables 4A and 5A on the web site.

References

  • Andersson, Henrik and Petter Lundborg. (2007). “Perception of Own Death Risk: An Analysis of Road-Traffic and Overall Mortality Risks,” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 34, 67–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Antoñanzas, Fernando, W. Kip Viscusi, Joan Rovira, Francisco J. Braña, Fabiola Portillo and Irineu Carvalho. (2000). “Smoking Risks in Spain: Part I—Perception of Risks to the Smoker,” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 21(2/3), 161–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arrow, Kenneth J. (1982). “Risk Perception in Psychology and Economics,” Economic Inquiry 20, 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beitel, George A., Michael Sharp, and William D. Glauz. (1975). “Probability of Arrest While Driving under the Influence of Alcohol,” Journal of Studies on Alcohol 36(1), 109–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benson, Bruce L. and David W. Rasmussen. (1999). “Deterring Drunk Driving Fatalities: An Economics of Crime Perspective,” International Review of Law and Economics 19, 205–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borkenstein, Robert F. (1975). “Problems of Enforcement, Adjudication and Sanctioning.” In S. Israelstam and S. Lambert (eds), Alcohol, Drugs and Traffic Safety. Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Alcohol, Drugs and Traffic Safety, Toronto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borkenstein, Robert F., R. F. Crowther, R. P. Shumate, W. B. Ziel, and R. Zylman. (1974). “The Role of the Drinking Driver in Traffic Accidents,” Blutalcohol 11(suppl. 1), 1–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourgeon, Jean-Marc and Pierre Picard. (2007). “Point-Record Driving Licence and Road Safety: An Economic Approach,” Journal of Public Economics 91, 235–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, Marcel and Georges Dionne. (1987). “The Economics of Road Safety,” Transportation Research 21B(5), 413–431.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, Stephen L. and Amy Cotton. (2003). “Risk-Mitigating Beliefs, Risk Estimates, and Self-Reporting Speeding in a Sample of Australian Drivers,” Journal of Safety Research 34, 183–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Combs, Barbara and Paul Slovic. (1979). “Causes of Death: Biased Newspaper Coverage and Biased Judgements,” Journalism Quarterly 56, 837–843.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeJoy, David. (1989). “The Optimism Bias and Traffic Accident Risk Perception,” Accident Analysis and Prevention 21(4), 333–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeJoy, David. (1992). “An Examination of Gender Differences in Traffic Accident Risk Perception,” Accident Analysis and Prevention 24, 237–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dionne, Georges, Jean Pinquet, Mathieu Maurice, and Charles Vanasse. (2007). Point-Record Incentives, Asymmetric Information and Dynamic Data. Mimeo, HEC Montréal and Ecole Polytechnique, France.

  • Eisenberg, Daniel. (2003). “Evaluating the Effectiveness of Policies Related to Drunk Driving,” Journal of Policy Analysis and Management 22(2), 249–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finn, P. and B. Bragg. (1986). “Perception of the Risk of an Accident by Young and Older Drivers,” Accident Analysis and Prevention 18, 289–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guerin, Bernard. (1994). “What Do People Think About the Risks of Driving? Implications for Traffic Safety Interventions,” Journal of Applied Social Psychology 24(11), 994–1021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hakes, Jahn K. and W. Kip Viscusi. (1997). “Mortality Risk Perceptions: A Bayesian Reassessment,” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 15(2), 135–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Homel, Ross. (1989). Policing and Punishing the Drinking Driver: A Study of General and Specific Deterrence. Springer-Verlag, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Job, R. F. Soames. (1990). “The Application of Learning Theory to Driving Confidence: The Effect of Age and the Impact of Random Breath Testing,” Accident Analysis and Prevention 22(2), 97–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenkel, Donald S. (1993). “Do Drunk Drivers Pay their Way? A Note on Optimal Penalties for Drunk Driving,” Journal of Health Economics 12, 137–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laberge-Nadeau, Claire, Urs Maag, François Bellavance, Denise Desjardins, Stéphane Messier, and Abdelnasser Saïdi. (2001). Wireless Telephones and the Risk of Road Accidents (Final report). Montréal: Centre for Research on Transportation, Laboratory on Transportation Safety, Université de Montréal, CRT-2001-16.

  • Levitt, Stephen D. and Jack Porter. (2001). “How Dangerous are Drunken Drivers?” Journal of Political Economy 109(6), 1198–1237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lichtenstein, Sarah, Paul Slovic, Baruch Fischhoff, Mark Layman, and Barbara Combs. (1978). “Judged Frequency of Lethal Events,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 4, 551–578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, Jin-Tan and Chee-Ruey Hsieh. (1995). “Risk Perception and Smoking Behavior: Empirical Evidence from Taiwan,” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 11, 139–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lundborg, Petter. (2007). “Smoking, Information Sources, and Risk Perceptions—New Results on Swedish Data,” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 34, 217–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magat, Wesley A., W. Kip Viscusi, and Joel Huber. (1987). “Risk–Dollar Tradeoffs, Risk Perceptions, and Consumer Behavior.” In W. Kip Viscusi and Wesley A. Magat (eds), Learning about Risk. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mannering, Fred L. and Lawrence L. Grodsky. (1995). “Statistical Analysis of Motorcyclists’ Perceived Accident Risks,” Accident Analysis and Prevention 27(1), 21–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, Michael L. and Andrew R. Moran. (1986). “Age Difference in Male Drivers’ Perception of Accident Risks,” Accident Analysis and Prevention 18(4), 299–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parker, Dianne, Antony S. Manstead, Stephen G. Stradling, and James T. Reason. (1992). “Intentions to Commit Driving Violations: An Application of the Theory of Planned Behavior,” Journal of Applied Psychology 77(1), 94–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paternoster, Raymond. (1987). “The Deterrent Effect of the Perceived Certainty and Severity of Punishment: A Review of the Evidence and Issues,” Justice Quarterly 4, 173–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phelps, Charles. (1987). “Risk and Perceived Risks of Drinking and Driving among Youths,” Journal of Policy Analysis and Management 6(4), 708–713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Polinsky, A. Mitchell and Steven Shavell. (2000). “The Economic Theory of Public Enforcement of Law,” Journal of Economic Literature 38, 45–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rafaely, Vered, Joachim Meyer, Ilena Silberman-Sandler, and Svetlana Viener. (2006). “Perception of Traffic Risks for Older and Younger Adults,” Accident Aanalysis and Prevention 38, 1231–1236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryb, Gabriel E., Patricia C. Dischinger, Joseph A. Kufera, and Kathy M. Read. (2006). “Risk Perception and Impulsivity: Association with Risky Behaviors and Substance Abuse Disorders,” Accident Analysis and Prevention 38, 567–573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shavell, Steven. (2004). Foundations of Economic Analysis of Law. The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

  • Slovic, Paul, Baruch Fischoff, and Sarah Lichtenstein. (1982). “Facts versus Fears: Understanding Perceived Risk.” In Daniel Kahneman, Paul Slovic, and Amos Tversky (eds), Judgement under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, V. Kerry and F. Reed Johnson. (1988). “How Do Risk Perceptions Respond to Information? The Case of Radon,” The Review of Economics and Statistics 70, 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, V. Kerry, William H. Desvousges, F. Reed Johnson, and Ann Fisher. (1990). “Can Public Information Programs Affect Risk Perceptions?” Journal of Policy Analysis and Management 9(1), 41–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SOM Inc. (1997). A Survey of Québec License Holders. Presented to the Communications Department of the Société de L’assurance Automobile du Québec for the Study of its “Alcohol 97” campaign.

  • Stasson, Mark and Martin Fishbein. (1990). “The Relation Between Perceived Risk and Preventive Action: A Within-Subject Analysis of Perceived Driving Risk and Intentions to Wear Seatbelts,” Journal of Applied Social Psychology 20, 1541–1557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Svenson, Ola, Baruch Fischhoff, and Donald MacGregor. (1985). “Perceived Driving Safety and Seatbelt Usage,” Accident Analysis and Prevention 17(2), 119–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vanlaar, Ward and George Yannis. (2006). “Perception of Road Accident Causes,” Accident Analysis and Prevention 38, 155–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Viscusi, W. Kip. (1985). “A Bayesian Perspective on Biases in Risk Perception,” Economics Letters 17, 59–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Viscusi, W. Kip. (1990). “Do Smokers Underestimate Risks?” Journal of Political Economy 98(6), 1253–1269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Viscusi, W. Kip. (1992). Smoking: Making the Risky Decision. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Viscusi, W. Kip and Charles J. O’Connor. (1984). “Adaptive Responses to Chemical Labeling: Are Workers Bayesian Decision Makers?” American Economic Review 74, 942–956.

    Google Scholar 

  • Viscusi, W. Kip, Irineu Carvalho, Fernando Antoñanzas, Joan Rovira, Francisco J. Braña and Fabiola Portillo. (2000). “Smoking Risks in Spain: Part III—Determinants of Smoking Behavior,” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 21(2/3), 213–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Voas, Robert B., JoAnn Wells, Diane Lestina, Allan Williams, and Michael Greene. (1998). “Drinking and Driving in the United States: the 1996 National Roadside Survey,” Accident Analysis and Prevention 30(2), 267–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, Douglas J. and Thomas W. Likens. (2000). “Alcohol Regulation and Auto Fatalities,” International Review of Law and Economics 20, 107–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zaal, Dominic. (1999). Traffic Law Enforcement: A Review of the Literature, Monash University, Institute for Road Safety Measure, Report no. 53.

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was financed by the Société de l’assurance automobile du Québec, the Québec Department of Transport, and the Fonds pour la formation et l’aide à la recherche in the FCAR-MTQ-SAAQ program on road safety. Previous versions have been presented at the École Nationale des Arts et Métiers, Paris, at the Risk Attitude Conference, Montpellier, France, and at the FUR XII Conference, Roma. We thank Jean Boudreault, Andrée Brassard, and Lyne Vézina for their collaboration at various stages of this project. Stéphane Messier made an excellent contribution to the preparation and the management of the survey and Claire Boisvert improved significantly the presentation of the original manuscript. We thank Michèle Cohen, the editor, and an anonymous referee for very useful comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Georges Dionne.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Dionne, G., Fluet, C. & Desjardins, D. Predicted risk perception and risk-taking behavior: The case of impaired driving. J Risk Uncertainty 35, 237–264 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11166-007-9023-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11166-007-9023-8

Keywords

JEL Classification

Navigation