Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Social Categorization on Perception Bias in the Practice of Microteaching

  • Published:
Research in Science Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Microteaching has gained considerable attention for its effectiveness in rapid and contextual training in professional development programs. However, the interpretive quality of the teaching demonstration and peer feedback may influence individuals’ attribution and self-correction, leading to ineffective learning. In this study, a microteaching workshop in a professional development program for 78 elementary school science teachers was investigated. The results showed that the effectiveness of microteaching was negatively affected by participants’ perception bias due to social categorization. Moreover, it was indicated that the participants’ perception of the in-group and out-group, classified by the degree of the individuals’ science knowledge, fostered social categorization. Participants tended to experience perception conflicts caused by their inability to see personal faults, and a typical perception bias of “seeing one’s own strengths and seeing others’ shortcomings” was more frequently recognized in the out-group. These results converge to highlight the importance of social categorization in perception bias relevant to microteaching.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ackerman, J. M., Shapiro, J. R., Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., Becker, D. V., Griskevicius, V., Maner, J. K., & Schaller, M. (2006). They all look the same to me (unless they’re angry): from out-group homogeneity to out-group heterogeneity. Psychological Science, 17, 836–840. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01790.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allen, D., & Ryan, K. (1969). Microteaching. Reading: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andres, J., & Mausfeld, R. (2008). Structural description and qualitative content in perception theory. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(1), 307–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bazerman, M. H. (2005). Judgment in managerial decision making (6th ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benton-Kupper, J. (2001). The microteaching experience: student perspectives. Education, 121(4), 830–835.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourgeois-Gironde, S. (2010). Is neuroeconomics doomed by the reverse inference fallacy? Mind & Society, 9(2), 229–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. (1997). Quantifying qualitative analyses of verbal data: a practical guide. Journal of Learning Sciences, 6, 271–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cottrell, C. A., & Neuberg, S. L. (2005). Different emotional reactions to different groups: a sociofunctional threat-based approach to “prejudice”. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 770–789.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crisp, R. J., & Hewstone, M. (2007). Multiple social categorization. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 39, pp. 163–254). San Diego: Elsevier Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crisp, R. J., Stone, C. H., & Hall, N. R. (2006). Recategorization and subgroup identification: predicting and preventing threats from common ingroups. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 230–243. doi:10.1177/0146167205280908.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Driscoll, M., & Moyer, J. (2001). Using students’ work as a lens on algebraic thinking. Mathematics Teaching in the Middle School, 6, 282–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Francis, D. (1997). Reconceptualizing microteaching as critical inquiry. Asia-Pacific Journal of Teacher Education, 25(3), 207–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, S. M., & Ybarra, O. (2007). People accounting: social category-based choice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(5), 802–809.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (1992). Analogical reasoning in children. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grimberg, E. I., & Gummer, E. (2013). Teaching science from cultural points of intersection. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 50(1), 12–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, D. L. (1979). A cognitive-attributional analysis of stereotyping. In In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 12, 53–84). New York: Academic Press.

  • Hogg, M. A. (2001). Social categorization, depersonalization, and group behavior. In M. A. Hogg & S. Tinsdale (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social psychology: group processes (pp. 56–85). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, J. H. (1973). Insight learning through structured observation of classroom interaction. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 10(3), 213–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (2002). Choices, values, and frames. Cambridge: Cambridge University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kong, S. C., Shroff, R. H., & Hung, H. K. (2009). A web enabled video system for self-reflection by student teachers using a guiding framework. Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 25(4), 544–558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, B. (1982). Cognitive development and the self. In J. M. Broughton & D. J. Freeman-Moir (Eds.), The cognitive developmental psychology of James Mark Baldwin: current theory and research in genetic epistemology (pp. 169–210). Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, L. (2011). Obstacles and opportunities for developing thinking through interaction in language classrooms. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 6(3), 146–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, M. (2012). Discussing teaching video-cases online: perspectives of preservice and inservice EFL teachers in Taiwan. Computers & Education, 59, 120–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macrae, C. N., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2000). Social cognition: thinking categorically about others. Annual Review of Psychology, 51(1), 93–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macrae, C. N., & Hewstone, M. R. C. (1990). Cognitive biases in social categorization: process and consequences. Advances in Psychology, 68, 325–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maitner, A. T., Mackie, D. M., & Smith, E. R. (2007). Antecedents and consequences of satisfaction and guilt following ingroup aggression. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 10, 223–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marin, L. M., & Halpern, D. F. (2011). Pedagogy for developing critical thinking in adolescents: explicit instruction produces greatest gains. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 6(1), 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meissner, C. A., & Brigham, J. C. (2001). Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces: a meta-analytic review. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7, 3–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mercer, N. (1996). The quality of talk in children’s collaborative activity in the classroom. Learning and Instruction, 6(4), 359–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mercer, N. (2004). Sociocultural discourse analysis: analyzing classroom talk as a social mode of thinking. Journal of Applied Linguistics, 1(2), 13–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MOE K12 Education Administration. (2013). Taiwan elementary and secondary educator community. Retrieved from http://teach.eje.edu.tw/.

  • Morrison, R. G. (2005). Thinking in working memory. In K. J. Holyoak & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 457–473). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, G. L. (2002). The big book of concepts. Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perlberg, A. (1987). Microteaching: conceptual and theoretical bases. In M. J. Dunkin (Ed.), The international encyclopedia of teaching and teacher education (pp. 715–720). New York: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pietraszewski, D., & Schwartz, A. (2014). Evidence that accent is a dimension of social categorization, not a byproduct of perceptual salience, familiarity, or ease-of-processing. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(1), 43–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Politzer, R. (1969). Microteaching: a new approach to teacher training and research. Hispania, 52(2), 244–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Popovich, N. G., & Katz, N. L. (2009). A microteaching exercise to develop performance-based abilities in pharmacy students. American Journal of Pharmacy Education, 73(4), 73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pronin, E. (2007). Perception and misperception of bias in human judgment. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(1), 37–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quellmalz, E. S., & Hoskyn, J. (1997). Classroom assessment of reasoning strategies. In G. D. Phye & J. Phye (Eds.), Handbook of classroom assessment: learning, adjustment, achievement. San Diego: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ray, D. G., & Matschke, C. (2012). Cross-group recognition bias generalizes to diverse non-face representations of digital identity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(1), 387–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ray, D. G., Way, N., & Hamilton, D. L. (2010). Crossed-categorization, evaluation, and face recognition. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(2), 449–452. doi:10.1016/j.jesp. 2009.12.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ray, D. G., Mackie, D. M., Smith, E. R., & Terman, A. W. (2012). Discrete emotions elucidate the effects of crossed-categorization on prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(1), 55–69. doi:10.1016/j.jesp. 2011.07.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Remesh, A. (2013). Microteaching, an efficient technique for learning effective teaching. Journal of Research in Medical Sciences, 18(2), 158–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reverberi, C., Shallice, T., D’Agostini, S., Skrap, M., & Bonatti, L. (2009). Cortical bases of elementary deductive reasoning: inference, memory, and meta-deduction. Neuropsychologia, 47(4), 1107–1116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richland, L. E., Chan, T. K., Morrison, R. G., & Au, T. K. F. (2010). Young children’s analogical reasoning across cultures: similarities and differences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105(1–2), 146–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richter, T., Schroeder, S., & Wohrmann, B. (2009). You don’t have to believe everything you read: background knowledge permits fast and efficient validation of information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(3), 538–558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rojas-Drummond, S., Torreblanca, O., Pedraza, H., Vélez, M., & Guzmán, K. (2013). ‘Dialogic scaffolding’: enhancing learning and understanding in collaborative contexts. Learning, Culture and Social Interaction, 2(1), 11–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, L., & Ward, A. (1996). Naïve realism in everyday life: implications for social conflict and misunderstanding. In T. Brown (Ed.), Values and knowledge: the Jean Piaget symposium series (pp. 103–135). Lawrence: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sani, F., Bennett, M., & Soutar, A. U. (2005). The ecological validity of the “who said what?” technique: an examination of the role of self-involvement, cognitive interference and acquaintanceship. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 46, 83–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scheepers, D., Spears, R., & Manstead, B. D. (2002). Integrating identity and instrumental approaches to intergroup differentiation: different contexts, different motives. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1455–1467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seger, C. R., Smith, E. R., & Mackie, D. M. (2009). Subtle activation of a social categorization triggers group-level emotions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(3), 460–467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sellars, N., & Francis, D. (1995). Collaborative professional development in a primary school: A case study. Journal of In-Service Education 21,(1), 15–36.

  • Smith, E. R., & Collins, E. C. (2009). Contextualizing person perception: distributed social cognition. Psychological Review, 116(2), 343–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2008). On the relative independence of thinking biases and cognitive ability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 672–695.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strawson, G. (2005). Intentionality and experience: terminological preliminaries. In D. Smith & A. Thomasson (Eds.), Phenomenology and philosophy of mind (pp. 41–66). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sussman, A. B., Khemlani, S. S., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2014). Latent scope bias in categorization. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 52, 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taggar, S., & Brown, T. C. (2006). Interpersonal affect and peer rating bias in teams. Small Group Research, 37(1), 86–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tajfel, H. (1982). Social psychology of intergroup relations. Annual Review of Psychology, 33, 1–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E. G., & Hummel, J. E. (2009). Finding similarity in a model of relational reasoning. Cognitive Systems Research, 10, 229–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, L., & Nadler, J. (2000). Judgmental biases in conflict resolution and how to overcome them. In M. Deutsch & P. T. Coleman (Eds.), The handbook of conflict resolution: theory and practice (pp. 213–235). New York: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2010). Groups in mind: the coalitional roots of war and morality. In H. Høgh-Olesen (Ed.), Human morality and sociality: evolutionary and comparative perspectives (pp. 91–234). New York: Palgrave MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D., & Wetherell, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the social group: a self-categorization theory. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Boven, L., White, K., Kamada, A., & Gilovich, T. (2003). Intuitions about situational correction in self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 249–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Leeuwen, F., Park, J. H., & Penton-Voak, I. S. (2012). Another fundamental social category? Spontaneous categorization of people who uphold or violate moral norms. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(6), 1385–1388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vescio, T. K., Judd, C. M., & Kwan, V. S. Y. (2004). The crossed-categorization hypothesis: evidence of reductions in the strength of categorization, but not intergroup bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 478–496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wajnryb, R. (1992). Classroom observation tasks: a resource book for language teachers and trainers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yeany, R. S. (1980). Redefining process and product in teacher training: alternative modes for evaluating teacher training programs. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 17(5), 383–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yeany, R. H., & Padilla, M. J. (1986). Training science teachers to utilize better teaching strategies: a research synthesis. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 23(2), 85–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, R. F. (2008). Language and interaction: an advanced resource book. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was partially supported by the “Aim for the Top University Project” of National Taiwan Normal University (NTNU), sponsored by the Ministry of Education, Taiwan and the “International Research-Intensive Center of Excellence Program” of NTNU and Ministry of Science and Technology, Taiwan (MOST 103-2911-I-003-301 and MOST 101-2511-S-003-056-MY3 and MOST 104-2911-I-003-301).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ming-Yueh Hwang.

Appendix. Microteaching Observation Sheet

Appendix. Microteaching Observation Sheet

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hong, JC., Hwang, MY., Lu, CC. et al. Social Categorization on Perception Bias in the Practice of Microteaching. Res Sci Educ 47, 185–201 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-015-9495-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-015-9495-5

Keywords

Navigation