Skip to main content
Log in

The Benefits of Mouse Keeping—an Empirical Study on Students’ Flow and Intrinsic Motivation in Biology Lessons

  • Published:
Research in Science Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Contact with living animals is an exceptional possibility within biology education to facilitate an intense immersion into the study topic and even allow for a flow experience (Csikszentmihalyi 2000). Further, it might affect the perceptions of the students’ basic needs for autonomy and competence and thereby their quality of motivation (Deci and Ryan 1985, 2002). Still, there is little empirical evidence about the duration of the exposure with living animals that is required. We investigated the students’ flow experience, and the students’ motivation, reported retrospectively in three different treatments: lessons involving short-term or long-term contact with living harvest mice and a control group without living animals. Our sample consisted of 156 fifth graders (10.76 years, SD = 0.513). The test instruments were adapted versions of the Flow Short Scale (FSS, Rheinberg et al. 2003) and of the Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI, Ryan 1982). As expected, the control group produced significantly lower scores for both FSS and IMI. In addition, we found a significant difference between students with short-term versus long-term contact. Whereas the flow experience was indistinguishable for all pupils who had contact with living animals, those with long-term experience reported significantly higher intrinsic motivation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bätz, K., Damerau, K., Lorenzen, S., & Wilde, M. (2011). Caring for animals as if caring for relationships?! – The effect of shared keeping of harvest mice in the classroom on students’ perception of their social relatedness. Berichte aus Institutionen der Didaktik der Biologie, 18, 43–52

  • Balmford, A., Clegg, L., Coulson, T., & Taylor, J. (2002). Why conservationists should heed pokémon. Science, 295(5564), 2367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chirkov, V., Ryan, R. M., Kim, Y., & Kaplan, U. (2003). Differentiating autonomy from individualism and independence: a self-determination theory perspective on internalization of cultural orientations and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 97–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: the psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Beyond boredom and anxiety: experiencing flow in work and play. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (Eds.). (1988). Optimal experience: psychological studies of flow in consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Schiefele, U. (1993). Quality of experience and the process of learning. Zeitschrift für Pädagogik, 39(2), 207–221.

  • Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and the “why” of human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11(4), 227–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2002). Handbook of self-determination research. Rochester: The University of Rochester.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2004). Self-determination theory and basic need satisfaction: understanding human development in positive psychology. Ricerche di Psichologia, 27, 17–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Düker, H., & Tausch, R. (1957). About the effect of illustration of subject matters on memorization. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie, 4, 384–400.

  • Engeser, S., & Rheinberg, F. (2008). Flow, moderators of challenge-skill-balance and performance. Motivation and Emotion, 32, 158–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gehlhaar, K. H. (2008). Living organisms. In H. Gropengießer & U. Kattmann (Eds.), Didactics of biology (pp. 298–311). Köln: Aulis.

  • Harackiewicz, J. M., & Hulleman, C. S. (2010). The importance of interest: the role of achievement goals and task values in promoting the development of interest. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(1), 42–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harel, I., & Papert, S. (1990). Software design as a learning environment. In I. Harel & S. Papert (Eds.), Constructionist learning (pp. 41–84). Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hidi, S. (2001). Interest and reading: theoretical and practical considerations. Educational Psychology Review, 13(3), 191–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase model of interest development. Educational Psychologist, 41, 111–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hidi, S., Renninger, K. A., & Krapp, A. (2004). Interest, a motivational construct that combines affective and cognitive functioning. In D. Y. Dai & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Motivation, emotion and cognition: integrative perspectives on intellectual functioning and development (pp. 89–115). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hummel, E., & Randler, C. (2010). Experiments with living animals–effects on learning success, experimental competency and emotions. Procedia–Social and Behavioral Sciences, 2, 3823–3830.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klingenberg, K. (2008). Effects of “Primärerfahrung” on interests, learning climate and attitudes: a comparative study with living animals and educational videos. 6th biennial ESERA Summerschool, Aug. 24-29th, York, GB.

  • Klingenberg, K. (2009a). Bridging the gap with “Primärerfahrung”: fostering affective changes as well as cognitive outcome by using living animals. 7th biennal ESERA-Conference, Aug. 31st –Sept. 04th, Istanbul, Turkey.

  • Klingenberg, K. (2009b). Wild living animals of protected species in school lessons – Specifications of the federal law on nature protection, the protection of animals act and the fishery legislation. Natur und Recht, 31(1), 32–40.

  • Klingenberg, K. (2012). Living animals in education. Analysen-Studien-Konzepte. Berlin: Logos.

  • Kowal, J., & Fortier, M. S. (1999). Motivational determinants of flow: contributions from self-determination theory. Journal of Social Psychology, 139, 355–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krapp, A. (2002). An educational-psychological theory of interest and its relation to self-determination theory. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), The handbook of self-determination research (pp. 405–427). Rochester: University of Rochester.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krapp, A. (2007). An educational-psychological conceptualisation of interest. International Journal of Educational and Vocational Guidance, 7(1), 5–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krapp, A., Schiefele, U., & Winteler, A. (1992). Interest as a predictor of academic achievement: a meta-analysis of research. In K. A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and development (pp. 183–196). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lock, R., & Millett, K. (1992). Using animals in education and research—student experience, knowledge and implications for teaching in the National Science Curriculum. School Science Review, 74(266), 115–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mietzel, G. (1998). Educational psychology of learning and teaching. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

  • Ministerium für Schule und Weiterbildung des Landes Nordrhein-Westfalen (2008). Curriculum for Gymnasium – Sekundarstufe I in North Rhine - Westphalia. Biology. School in NRW, 3308.

  • Mitchell, M. (1993). Situational interest: its multifaceted structure in the secondary school mathematics classroom. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(3), 424–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Science Teachers Association (NSTA). (2005). Responsible use of live animals and dissection in the science classroom. NSTA Board of Directors, June 2005, Revised: March 2008.

  • Niemiec, C. P., Lynch, M. F., Vansteenkiste, M., Bernstein, J., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2006). The antecedents and consequences of autonomous self-regulation for college: a self-determination theory perspective on socialization. Journal of Adolescence, 29, 761–775.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prokop, P., Tuncer, G., & Chudá, J. (2007). Slovakian students’ attitudes toward biology. Eurasia Journal of Mathematics, Science and Technology Education, 3(4), 287–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prokop, P., Prokop, M., & Tunnicliffe, S. D. (2008). Effects of keeping animals as pets on children’s concepts of vertebrates and invertebrates. International Journal of Science Education, 30(4), 431–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberg, F. (2008). Intrinsic motivation and flow. In J. Heckhausen & H. Heckhausen (Eds.), Motivation and action (pp. 323–348). Cambridge: Cambridge University.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberg, F., Vollmeyer, R., & Engeser, S. (2003). The recording of Flow-Experience. In J. Stiensmeier- Pelster & F. Rheinberg (Eds.), Diagnostics of motivation and self-conception (pp. 261–279). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

  • Ryan, R. M. (1982). Control and information in the intrapersonal sphere: an extension of cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 450–461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., Conneell, J. P., & Plant, R. W. (1990). Emotions in non-directed text learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 2, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2002). An overview of self-determination theory. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 3–33). Rochester: University of Rochester.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, W. L., & Young, G. D. (1985). An experimental study of the effect of the presence or absence of living visual aids in high school biology classrooms upon attitudes towards science and biology achievement. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 22(7), 619–629.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schüler, J. (2007). Arousal of flow-experience in a learning setting and its effects on exam-performance and affect. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 21, 217–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shernoff, D. J., Csikszentmihalyi, M., Schneider, B., & Shernoff, E. S. (2003). Student engagement in high school classrooms from the perspective of flow theory. School Psychology Quarterly, 18, 158–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherwood, K. P., Rallis, S. F., & Stone, I. (1989). Effects of live animals vs preserved specimens on student learning. Zoo Biology, 8, 99–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schraw, G., & Lehman, S. (2001). Situational interest: a review of the literature and directions for future research. Educational Psychology Review, 13, 23–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tamir, P., & Shcurr, Y. (1997). Back to living animals. Journal of Biological Education, 31(4), 300–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tausch, R., & Tausch, A. M. (1963). Educational Psychology. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

  • Vallerand, R. J., & Ratelle, C. F. (2002). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: a hierarchical model. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 37–64). Rochester: University of Rochester.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: the concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilde, M., & Bätz, K. (2009). They are so sweeeet! – The influence of educational use of living harvest mice on knowledge acquisition, motivation and the demand for animal keeping. Berichte aus Institutionen der Didaktik der Biologie, 17, 19–30.

  • Wilde, M., Bätz, K., Kovaleva, A., & Urhahne, D. (2009). Examination of a short scale for intrinsic motivation (KIM). Zeitschrift für Didaktik der Naturwissenschaften, 15, 31–45.

  • Wilde, M., Hussmann, J., Lorenzen, S., Meyer, A., & Randler, C. (2012). Lessons with living harvest mice. An empirical study of their effects on intrinsic motivation and knowledge acquisition. International Journal of Science Education, 34(18), 2797–2810.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, O. E. (1984). Biophilia. Cambridge: Harvard University.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Matthias Wilde.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Meyer, A., Klingenberg, K. & Wilde, M. The Benefits of Mouse Keeping—an Empirical Study on Students’ Flow and Intrinsic Motivation in Biology Lessons. Res Sci Educ 46, 79–90 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-014-9455-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-014-9455-5

Keywords

Navigation