Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Reading fluency from grade 2–6: a longitudinal examination

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The goal of this study was to examine oral word reading fluency from a developmental perspective in a longitudinal study of students from second grade to sixth grade. The sample was consisted of native English speaking students that took part in a large longitudinal study. Participants were assessed on cognitive and literacy measures such as working memory, phonological awareness, rapid automatized naming and syntactic awareness—oral cloze. Two main research questions were examined: first, what relationships will be found between the cognitive, literacy and linguistic measures, and which of them simultaneously predict oral reading fluency, in each age group? And second, which cognitive and literacy measures in second grade predict word reading fluency in sixth grade? Results show that cognitive and literacy measures contribute differently to word reading fluency skill across the different grades, while the only strong predictor across all age groups, was the phonological awareness. Finally, taking together past and previous findings, a proposed definition of fluency from a developmental perspective is suggested, based on the results of the study that show in a clear manner, that reading fluency, its contributors and its predictors, change respectively to the reading phase obtained in each grade.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anthony, J. L., & Francis, D. J. (2005). Development of phonological awareness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(5), 255–259. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00376.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, J., Dix, H., Bontrager, M., Dey, R., & Archer, A. (2013). Phonemic awareness contributes to text reading fluency: Evidence from eye movements. School Psychology Review, 42(2), 157–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, C. A. (1995). Educating for an ecologically sustainable culture: Rethinking moral education, creativity, intelligence, and other modern orthodoxies. Albany: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cardoso-Martins, C., & Pennington, B. F. (2004). The relationship between phoneme awareness and rapid serial naming skills and literacy acquisition: The role of developmental period and reading ability. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8(1), 27–52. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0801_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Case, R., Kurland, D. M., & Goldberg, J. (1982). Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33(3), 386–404. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(82)90054-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christopher, M. E., Miyake, A., Keenan, J. M., Pennington, B., DeFries, J. C., Wadsworth, S. J., et al. (2012). Predicting word reading and comprehension with executive function and speed measures across development: A latent variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(3), 470. doi:10.1037/a0027375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M. (1991). Working memory as a predictor of verbal fluency. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 20(6), 445–464. doi:10.1007/bf01067637.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19(4), 450–466. doi:10.1016/s0022-5371(80)90312-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D'angiulli, A., Siegel, L. S., & Maggi, S. (2004). Literacy instruction, SES, and word-reading achievement in english-language learners and children with english as a first language: A longitudinal study. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 19(4), 202–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid ‘automatized’ naming (RAN): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14(4), 471–479. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(76)90075-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Firm Foundations: Early literacy teaching and training. (2001). North Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada: School District No. 44. Retrieved from http://www.nvsd44.bc.ca/FirmFoundations/main.html.

  • Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Hosp, M. K., & Jenkins, J. R. (2001). Oral reading fluency as an indicator of reading competence: A theoretical, empirical, and historical analysis. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5(3), 239–256. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0503_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Phonological working memory: A critical building block for reading development and vocabulary acquisition? European Journal of Psychology Education, 8(3), 259–272. doi:10.1007/bf03174081.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Good, R. H., III, Simmons, D. C., & Kame’enui, E. J. (2001). The importance and decision-making utility of a continuum of fluency-based indicators of foundational reading skills for third-grade high-stakes outcomes. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5(3), 257–288. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0503_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, J. R., Fuchs, L. S., Van Den Broek, P., Espin, C., & Deno, S. L. (2003). Sources of individual differences in reading comprehension and reading fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(4), 719–729. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.719.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katzir, T., Kim, Y., Wolf, M., O’Brien, B., Kennedy, B., Lovett, M., et al. (2006). Reading fluency: The whole is more than the parts. Annals of Dyslexia, 56(1), 51–82. doi:10.1007/s11881-006-0003-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katzir, T., Schiff, R., & Kim, Y. S. (2012). The effects of orthographic consistency on reading development: A within and between cross-linguistic study of fluency and accuracy among fourth grade English-and Hebrew-speaking children. Learning and Individual Differences, 22(6), 673–679. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2012.07.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y. S., Petscher, Y., Schatschneider, C., & Foorman, B. (2010). Does growth rate in oral reading fluency matter in predicting reading comprehension achievement? Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(3), 652–667. doi:10.1037/a0019643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, D., Park, Y., & Lombardino, L. J. (2015). Rapid automatized naming, word-level reading, and oral reading fluency in first-grade Korean readers at risk for reading difficulties. Asia Pacific Education Review, 1–13.

  • Klauda, S. L., & Guthrie, J. T. (2008). Relationships of three components of reading fluency to reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(2), 310–321. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.100.2.310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koponen, T., Salmi, P., Eklund, K., & Aro, T. (2013). Counting and RAN: Predictors of arithmetic calculation and reading fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(1), 162–175. doi:10.1037/a0029285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, M. R., Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Meisinger, E. B. (2010). Aligning theory and assessment of reading fluency: Automaticity, prosody, and definitions of fluency. Reading Research Quarterly, 45(2), 230–251. doi:10.1598/rrq.45.2.4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, M. R., & Stahl, S. A. (2003). Fluency: A review of developmental and remedial practices. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(1), 3–21. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293–323. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(74)90015-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2008). Development of word reading fluency and spelling in a consistent orthography: An 8-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(1), 150–161. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.100.1.150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macdonald, H. H., Sullivan, A. L., & Watkins, M. W. (2013). Multivariate screening model for later word reading achievement: Predictive utility of prereading skills and cognitive ability. Journal of Applied School Psychology, 29(1), 52–71. doi:10.1080/15377903.2013.751476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mokhtari, K., & Thompson, H. B. (2006). How problems of reading fluency and comprehension are related to difficulties in syntactic awareness skills among fifth graders. Literacy Research and Instruction, 46(1), 73–94. doi:10.1080/19388070609558461.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, R. D., Stuebing, K. K., Fletcher, J. M., Shaywitz, S. E., Lyon, G. R., Shankweiler, D. P., et al. (1998). Subtypes of reading disability: Variability around a phonological core. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(3), 347–373. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.90.3.347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Reading Panel. (2000). Report of the national reading panel: Teaching children to read. Report of the subgroups. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, National Institutes of Health.

  • Papadimitriou, A. M., & Vlachos, F. M. (2014). Which specific skills developing during preschool years predict the reading performance in the first and second grade of primary school? Early Child Development and Care, 184(11), 1706–1722. doi:10.1080/03004430.2013.875542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., Cardoso-Martins, C., Green, P. A., & Lefly, D. L. (2001). Comparing the phonological and double deficit hypotheses for developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 707–755. doi:10.1023/A:1012239018038.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petscher, Y., & Kim, Y. S. (2011). The utility and accuracy of oral reading fluency score types in predicting reading comprehension. Journal of School Psychology, 49(1), 107–129. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2010.09.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rasinski, T. V. (2012). Why reading fluency should be hot! The Reading Teacher, 65(8), 516–522. doi:10.1002/trtr.01077.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reading 44: A core reading framework. (1999). North Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada: School District No. 44. Retrieved from http://www.nvsd44.bc.ca/Reading44/main.html.

  • Rodríguez, C., van den Boer, M., Jiménez, J. E., & de Jong, P. F. (2015). Developmental changes in the relations between RAN, phonological awareness, and reading in Spanish children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 19(4), 273–288. doi:10.1080/10888438.2015.1025271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roehrig, A. D., Petscher, Y., Nettles, S. M., Hudson, R. F., & Torgesen, J. K. (2008). Accuracy of the DIBELS oral reading fluency measure for predicting third grade reading comprehension outcomes. Journal of School Psychology, 46(3), 343–366. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2007.06.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E. (1994). Toward an interactive model of reading. Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Samuels, S. J. (2012). Reading fluency: Its past, present and future. In T. Rasinski, C. Blachowicz, & K. Lems (Eds.), Fluency instruction: Research-based best practices (2nd ed., pp. 3–16). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwanenflugel, P. J., Hamilton, A. M., Kuhn, M. R., Wisenbaker, J. M., & Stahl, S. A. (2004). Becoming a fluent reader: Reading skill and prosodic features in the oral reading of young readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(1), 119–129. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sesma, H. W., Mahone, E. M., Levine, T., Eason, S. H., & Cutting, L. E. (2009). The contribution of executive skills to reading comprehension. Child Neuropsychology, 15(3), 232–246. doi:10.1080/09297040802220029.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S. (1994). Working memory and reading: A life-span perspective. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 17(1), 109–124. doi:10.1177/016502549401700107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S., & Ryan, E. B. (1989). The development of working memory in normally achieving and subtypes of learning disabled children. Child Development, 60(4), 973–980. doi:10.2307/1131037.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Solari, E. J., Aceves, T. C., Higareda, I., Richards-Tutor, C., Filippini, A. L., Gerber, M. M., et al. (2014). Longitudinal prediction of 1st- and 2nd-grade English oral reading fluency in English language learners: Which early reading and language skills are better predictors? Psychology in the Schools, 51(2), 126–142. doi:10.1002/pits.21743.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, L. H., Howard, C. B., & Saez, L. (2006). Do differences in components of working memory underlie different subgroups of reading disabilities? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39(3), 252–269. doi:10.1177/00222194060390030501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tobia, V., & Marzocchi, G. M. (2014). Predictors of reading fluency in Italian orthography: Evidence from a cross-sectional study of primary school students. Child Neuropsychology, 20(4), 449–469. doi:10.1080/09297049.2013.814768.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaessen, A., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D., Csépe, V., Faísca, L., Reis, A., et al. (2010). Cognitive development of fluent word reading does not qualitatively differ between transparent and opaque orthographies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(4), 827–842. doi:10.1037/a0019465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaessen, A., & Blomert, L. (2010). Long-term cognitive dynamics of fluent reading development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105(3), 213–231. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.11.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valencia, S. W., Smith, A. T., Reece, A. M., Li, M., Wixson, K. K., & Newman, H. (2010). Oral reading fluency assessment: Issues of construct, criterion, and consequential validity. Reading Research Quarterly, 45(3), 270–291. doi:10.1598/rrq.45.3.1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, G. S. (1993). The wide range achievement test 3. Wilmington, DE: Jastak Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willows, D. M., & Ryan, E. B. (1986). The development of grammatical sensitivity and its relationship to early reading achievement. Reading Research Quarterly, 21(3), 253–266. doi:10.2307/747708.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Katzir-Cohen, T. (2001). Reading fluency and its intervention. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5(3), 211–239. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0503_2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock reading mastery tests, revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D., Csépe, V., Reis, A., Faísca, L., et al. (2010). Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading: A cross-language investigation. Psychological Science, 21(4), 551–559. doi:10.1177/0956797610363406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by grants from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada to Linda S. Siegel. The author would like to thank the principals, teachers, and students of the North Vancouver School District for their support and participation in this study. The author would like to thank Linda Siegel for granting access to the data base as well as her ongoing support.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Orly Lipka.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lipka, O. Reading fluency from grade 2–6: a longitudinal examination. Read Writ 30, 1361–1375 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9729-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9729-1

Keywords

Navigation