Skip to main content
Log in

Lexical processes in the recognition of Japanese horizontal and vertical compounds

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

A Correction to this article was published on 11 October 2018

This article has been updated

Abstract

This lexical decision eye-tracking study investigated whether horizontal and vertical readings elicit comparable behavioral patterns and whether reading directions modulate lexical processes. Response times and eye movements were recorded during a lexical decision task with Japanese bimorphemic compound words presented vertically. The data were then analyzed together with those obtained in a horizontal lexical decision experiment of Miwa, Libben, Dijkstra, and Baayen (2014). Linear mixed-effects analyses of response times and eye movements revealed that, although response times and first fixation durations were notably shorter in horizontal reading than vertical reading, the vertical reading elicited fewer fixations. Furthermore, while compounds were recognized largely in comparable ways regardless of reading direction, several lexical processes were found to be reading-direction-dependent. Particularly, processing of the first morpheme was modulated by reading direction in a late time frame, such that a horizontal reading advantage was observed for words with a high frequency first morpheme. All in all, the two reading directions do not only differ quantitatively in processing speed, but also qualitatively in terms of underlying processing mechanisms.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Change history

  • 11 October 2018

    The original publication of the article contained an error

References

  • Amano, S., & Kondo, T. (2003). NTT database series: Lexical properties of Japanese, 2nd release [CD-ROM]. Tokyo: Sanseido.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H. (2013). languageR: Data sets and functions with “Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics”. R package version 1.4.1. Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=languageR.

  • Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H., Dijkstra, T., & Schreuder, R. (1997). Singulars and plurals in Dutch: Evidence for a parallel dual-route model. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 94–117. doi:10.1006/jmla.1997.2509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255–278. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S., & Baayen, R. H. (2015). Parsimonious mixed models. Manuscript submitted for publication. Retrieved from http://arxiv.org/abs/1506.04967.

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B. (2011). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999375-42. Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.

  • Bertram, R., & Hyönä, J. (2003). The length of a complex word modifies the role of morphological structure: Evidence from eye movements when reading short and long Finnish compounds. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 615–634. doi:10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00539-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Box, G. E. P., & Cox, D. R. (1964). An analysis of transformations (with discussion). Journal of the Royal Statistical Society B, 26, 211–252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donders, F. C. (1969). On the speed of mental processes. Acta Psychologica, 30, 412-431. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(69)90065-1. (Original work published in 1868)

  • Feldman, L. B., & Siok, W. W. T. (1997). The role of component function in visual recognition of Chinese characters. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 776–781. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.23.3.776.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K. I. (2000). The potential for experimenter bias effects in word recognition experiments. Memory & Cognition, 28, 1109–1115. doi:10.3758/BF03211812.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giraudo, H., & Grainger, J. (2001). Priming complex words: Evidence for supralexical representation of morphology. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 127–131. doi:10.3758/BF03196148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C., & Sandoval, T. C. (2008). More use almost always means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 787–814. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.07.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hsiao, J. H., & Cottrell, G. W. (2009). Not all visual expertise is holistic, but it may be leftist. Psychological Science, 20, 455–463. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02315.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huey, E. B. (1898). Preliminary experiments in the physiology and psychology of reading. The American Journal of Psychology, 9, 575–586. doi:10.2307/1412192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyönä, J., & Bertram, R. (2004). Do frequency characteristics of nonfixated words influence the processing of fixated words during reading? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 104–127. doi:10.1080/09541440340000132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyöna, J., Laine, M., & Niemi, J. (1995). Effects of a word’s morphological complexity on readers’ eye fixation patterns. Studies in Visual Information Processing, 6, 445–452. doi:10.1016/S0926-907X(05)80037-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Igawa, M., Nakayama, N., Maeda, F., & Tabuchi, A. (2006). Tategaki yokogaki bunsho ni okeru dokushoji no gankyuundo no hikaku [Differences in eye movements during reading vertically and horizontally printed sentences]. Rinsho Ganka, 60, 1251–1255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inhoff, A. W., Pollatsek, A., Posner, M. I., & Rayner, K. (1989). Covert attention and eye movements during reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 41, 63–89. doi:10.1080/14640748908402353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kajii, N., Nazir, T. A., & Osaka, N. (2001). Eye movement control in reading unspaced text: The case of the Japanese script. Vision Research, 41, 2503–2510. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00132-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuperman, V., Schreuder, R., Bertram, R., & Baayen, R. H. (2009). Reading of polymorphemic Dutch compounds: Towards a multiple route model of lexical processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 876–895. doi:10.1037/a0013484.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2015). lmerTest: Tests in linear mixed effects models. R package version 2.0-25. Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lmerTest.

  • Lavidor, M., Babkoff, H., & Faust, M. (2001). Analysis of standard and non-standard visual word format in the two hemispheres. Neuropsychologia, 39, 430–439. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00125-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Libben, G., & Jarema, G. (2002). Mental lexicon research in the new millennium. Brain and Language, 81, 1–10. doi:10.1006/brln.2002.2654.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maekawa, K., Yamazaki, M., Ogiso, T., Maruyama, T., Ogura, H., Kashino, W., et al. (2014). Balanced corpus of contemporary written Japanese. Language Resources & Evaluation, 48, 345–371. doi:10.1007/s10579-013-9261-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martinez-Conde, S., Otero-Millan, J., & Macknik, S. L. (2014). The impact of micro saccades on vision: Towards a unified theory of saccadic function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14, 83–96. doi:10.1038/nrn3405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matuschek, H., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S., Baayen, H., & Bates, D. (2015). Balancing type i error and power in linear mixed models. Manuscript submitted for publication. Retrieved from http://arxiv.org/abs/1511.01864.

  • Miwa, K., Libben, G., & Baayen, R. H. (2012). Semantic radicals in Japanese two-character word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27, 142–158. doi:10.1080/01690965.2011.552339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miwa, K., Libben, G., Dijkstra, T., & Baayen, R. H. (2014). The time-course of lexical activation in Japanese morphographic word recognition: Evidence for a character-driven processing model. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 79–113. doi:10.1080/17470218.2013.790910.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Institute for Japanese Language and Linguistics. (2012). Balanced corpus of contemporary written Japanese: Shonagon. Retrieved from http://www.kotonoha.gr.jp/shonagon/.

  • National Institute for Japanese Language and Linguistics. (2013). BCCWJ word list. Retrieved from http://pj.ninjal.ac.jp/corpus_center/bccwj/freq-list.html.

  • Osaka, N. (1989). Eye fixation and saccade during kana and kanji text reading: Comparison of English and Japanese text processing. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 27, 548–550. doi:10.3758/BF03334665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osaka, N., & Oda, K. (1991). Effective visual field size necessary for vertical reading during Japanese text processing. Bulletin of Psychonomic Society, 29, 345–347. doi:10.3758/BF03333939.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • R Development Core Team. (2015). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from http://www.R-project.org.

  • Rogers, H. (2005). Writing systems: A linguistic approach. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology: Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science, 22, 1359–1366. doi:10.1177/0956797611417632.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Staub, A., Rayner, K., Pollatsek, A., & Hyönä, J. (2007). The time course of plausibility effects on eye movements in reading: Evidence from noun-noun compounds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 1162–1169. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.6.1162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steinman, R. M., Haddad, G. M., Skavenski, A. A., & Wyman, D. (1973). Miniature eye movement. Science, 181, 810–819. doi:10.1126/science.181.4102.810.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M., & Forster, K. (1975). Lexical storage and retrieval of prefixed words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 638–647. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(75)80051-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M., & Zhu, X. (1997). Submorphemic processing in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 761–775. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.23.3.761.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M., Zhu, X., & Peng, D. (1999). Positional specificity of radicals in Chinese character recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 498–519. doi:10.1006/jmla.1998.2625.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tamaoka, K., & Hatsuzuka, M. (1998). The effects of morphological semantics on the processing of Japanese two-kanji compound words. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 10, 293–322. doi:10.1007/978-94-015-9161-4_8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tamaoka, K., & Kiyama, S. (2013). The effects of visual complexity for Japanese kanji processing with high and low frequencies. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 26, 205–223. doi:10.1007/s11145-012-9363-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tamaoka, K., & Makioka, S. (2004). New figures for a web-accessible database of the 1,945 basic Japanese kanji, fourth edition. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 548–558. doi:10.3758/BF03195601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tamaoka, K., & Takahashi, N. (1999). The effects of word frequency and orthographic complexity on the writing process of Japanese two-morpheme compound words. The Japanese Journal of Psychology, 70, 45–50. doi:10.4992/jjpsy.70.45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tinker, M. (1955). Perceptual and oculomotor efficiency in reading materials in vertical and horizontal arrangements. The American Journal of Psychology, 68, 444–449. doi:10.2307/1418529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Venables, W. N., & Ripley, B. D. (2002). Modern applied statistics with S (4th ed.). New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wickham, H. (2009). ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wurm, L. H., & Fisicaro, S. A. (2014). What residualizing predictors in regression analyses does (and what it does not do). Journal of Memory and Language, 72, 37–48. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2013.12.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yu, D., Park, H., Gerold, D., & Legge, G. E. (2010). Comparing reading speed for horizontal and vertical English text. Journal of Vision, 10, 1–17. doi:10.1167/10.2.21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Izaak Walton Killam scholarship from the Killam Trusts to the first author. Response time and eye movement data after data trimming, accompanying participant and item properties, will be available on the first author’s website (http://www.kojimiwa.com/publication.html). The frequency data collected from the Balanced Corpus of Contemporary Written Japanese (BCCWJ) are published with permission of the National Institute for Japanese Language and Linguistics. Authors thank Victor Kuperman and anonymous reviewers for their comments on an earlier version of this paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Koji Miwa.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Miwa, K., Dijkstra, T. Lexical processes in the recognition of Japanese horizontal and vertical compounds. Read Writ 30, 791–812 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9700-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9700-6

Keywords

Navigation