Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

That noun phrase may be beneficial and this may not be: discourse cohesion in reading and writing

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper examines the effects of attended and unattended demonstratives on text processing, comprehension, and writing quality in two studies. In the first study, participants (n = 45) read 64 mini-stories in a self-paced reading task and identified the main referent in the clauses. The sentences varied in the type of demonstratives (i.e., this, that, these, and those) contained in the sentences and whether the referent was followed by a demonstrative determiner and noun (i.e., an attended demonstrative) or a demonstrative pronoun (i.e., an unattended demonstrative). In the second study, 173 persuasive essays written by high school students were rated by expert judges on overall writing quality using a standardized rubric. Expert coders manually counted the number and types of demonstratives (attended and unattended demonstratives) in each essay. These counts were used to predict the human scores of essay quality. The findings demonstrate that the use of unattended demonstratives as anaphoric references is disadvantageous to both reading time and referent identification. However, these disadvantages become advantages in terms of essay quality likely because linguistic complexity is a strong indicator of high proficiency writing. From a text processing and comprehension viewpoint, the findings indicate, then, that anaphoric reference is not always beneficial and does not always create a more cohesive text. In contrast, from a writing context, the use of unattended demonstratives leads to a more linguistically complex text, which generally equates to a higher quality text.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The SAT is a college entrance exam commonly administered in the United States. An important component of the exam is a writing section in which test-takers are required to produce an essay based on general knowledge within a 25-min time frame.

References

  • Amaral, M. P. (1985). On the categories of textual cohesion and text complexity. Letras De Hoje, 18(60), 29–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychological Association. (2001). Publication manual of the American Psychological Association (6th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, A., Garrod, S., & Sanford, A. J. (1983). The accessibility of pronominal antecedents as a function of episode shifts in narrative text. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35, 427–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Axelrod, R. B., & Cooper, C. R. (2008). The St Martin’s guide to writing (8th ed.). Boston: Bedford/St Martin’s.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B. (2014). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using s4 classes.

  • Burstein, J., Chodorow, M., & Leacock, C. (2004). Automated essay evaluation: The Criterion online service. AI Magazine, 25, 27–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buswell, G. T. (1922). Fundamental reading habits: A study of their development. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H., & Sengul, C. J. (1979). In search of referents for nouns and pronouns. Memory and Cognition, 7, 35–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Connor, U. (1990). Linguistic/rhetorical measures of international persuasive student writing. Research in the Teaching of English, 24, 67–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crossley, S. A., Allen, L. K., Kyle, K., & McNamara, D. S. (2014). Analyzing discourse processing using the simple natural language processing tool (SiNLP). Discourse Processes, 51(5–6), 511–534.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crossley, S. A., Cai, Z., & McNamara, D. S. (2012). Syntagmatic, paradigmatic, and automatic n-gram approaches to assessing essay quality. In P. M. McCarthy & G. M. Youngblood (Eds.), Proceedings of the 25th international Florida Artificial Intelligence Research Society (FLAIRS) conference. (pp. 214–219). Menlo Park, CA: The AAAI Press.

  • Crossley, S. A., Kyle, K., & McNamara, D. S. (2015). To aggregate or not? Linguistic features in automatic essay scoring and feedback systems. Journal of Writing Assessment, 8(1).

  • Crossley, S. A., Kyle, K., & McNamara, D. S. (in press). The Tool for the Automatic Analysis of Text Cohesion (TAACO): Automatic assessment of local, global, and text cohesion. Behavior Research Methods.

  • Crossley, S. A., & McNamara, D. S. (2010). Cohesion, coherence, and expert evaluations of writing proficiency. In S. Ohlsson & R. Catrambone (Eds.), Proceedings of the 32nd annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 984–989). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.

  • Crossley, S. A., & McNamara, D. S. (2011). Text coherence and judgments of essay quality: Models of quality and coherence. In L. Carlson, C. Hoelscher, & T. F. Shipley (Eds.), Proceedings of the 29th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 1236–1241). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.

  • Crossley, S. A., Roscoe, R., & McNamara, D. S. (2013). Using automatic scoring models to detect changes in student writing in an intelligent tutoring system. In McCarthy, P. M. & Youngblood G. M., (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th international Florida Artificial Intelligence Research Society (FLAIRS) conference. (pp. 208–213). Menlo Park, CA: The AAAI Press.

  • Crossley, S. A., Weston, J., McLain Sullivan, S. T., & McNamara, D. S. (2011). The development of writing proficiency as a function of grade level: A linguistic analysis. Written Communication, 28(3), 282–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davies, M. (2009). The 385+ million word Corpus of Contemporary American English (1990–2008+): Design, architecture, and linguistic insights. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 14(2), 159–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dell, G. S., McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1983). The activation of antecedent information during the processing of anaphoric reference in reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 121–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doyle, A. E. (1982). The limitations of Cohesion. Research in the Teaching of English, 16(4), 390–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ede, L. (2004). Work in progress (6th ed.). Boston: Bedford/St. Martins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehrlich, K., & Rayner, K. (1983). Pronouns assignment and semantic integration during reading: Eye-movements and immediacy of processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 75–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Faigley, L. (2007). Writing: A guide for college and beyond. New York: Pearson/Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finn, S. (1995). Measuring effective writing: Cloze procedure and anaphoric “this”. Written Communication, 12(2), 240–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garrod, S., Freudenthal, D., & Boyle, E. (1994). The role of different types of anaphor in the on-line resolution of sentences in a discourse. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 39–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garrod, S., & Sanford, A. (1977). Interpreting anaphoric relations: The integration of semantic information while reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 77–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geisler, C., Kaufer, D., & Steinberg, E. (1985). The unattended anaphoric “this”: When should writers use it? Written Communication, 2(2), 129–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M. A. (1989). Mechanisms that improve referential access. Cognition, 32, 99–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, B., & Cortes, V. (2011). Perception vs. evidence: An analysis of this and these in academic prose. English for Specific Purposes, 30(1), 31–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M. A. K., & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Himmelman, N. P. (1996). Demonstratives in narrative discourse: A taxonomy of universal uses. In B. Fox (Ed.), Studies in Anaphora (pp. 205–254). Amsterdam: John Benjamin.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, S. B., Tanenhaus, M. K. & Dell, G. S. (1986). The effect of discourse center on the local coherence of a discourse. In Proceedings of the eighth annual conference of the cognitive science society, (pp. 96–101). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates

  • Johnson-Sheehan, R. (2005). Technical communication today. New York: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1987). The psychology of reading and language comprehension. Boston: Allyn & Bacon Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1985). Language and cognitive processes from a developmental perspective. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1(1), 61–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kossowska, A. (2004). The anaphora-cataphora switch using pronouns, determinatives, and adverbs. Studia Linguistica, 23, 29–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kyle, K., & Crossley, S. A. (2015). Automatically Assessing Lexical Sophistication: Indices, Tools, Findings, and Application. TESOL Quarterly, 49(4), 757–786.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loxterman, J. A., Beck, I. L., & McKeown, M. G. (1994). The effects of thinking aloud during reading on students’ comprehension of more or less coherent text. Reading Research Quarterly, 29, 353–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, J. (1977). Semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, M., & MacWhinney, B. (1990). Measuring inhibition and facilitation from pronouns. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 469–492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markel, M. (2004). Technical communication (7th ed.). Boston: Bedford/St. Martins.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCulley, G. A. (1985). Writing quality, coherence, and cohesion. Research in the Teaching of English, 19(3), 269–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1980). The comprehension processes and memory structures involved in anaphoric reference. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 668–682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, D. S., Crossley, S. A., & McCarthy, P. M. (2010). The linguistic features of quality writing. Written Communication, 27(1), 57–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, D. S., Crossley, S. A., & Roscoe, R. (2013). Natural language processing in an intelligent writing strategy tutoring system. Behavior Research Methods, 45(2), 499–515.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, D. S., & Kintsch, W. (1996). Learning from text: Effects of prior knowledge and text coherence. Discourse Processes, 22, 247–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, D. S., Kintsch, E., Songer, N. B., & Kintsch, W. (1996). Are good texts always better? Text coherence, background knowledge, and levels of understanding in learning from text. Cognition and Instruction, 14, 1–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mirman, D. (2014). Growth curve analysis and visualization using R. New York: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (1994). The psychology of reading. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Römer, U., & Wulff, S. (2010). Applying corpus methods to writing research: Explorations of MICUSP. Journal of Writing Research, 2(2), 99–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rustipa, K. (2015). The use of demonstrative pronoun and demonstrative determiner this in upper-level student writing: A case study. English Language Teaching, 8(5), 158–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, A. M. (2004). How including prior knowledge as a subject variable may change outcomes of learning research. American Educational Research Journal, 41(1), 159–189. doi:10.3102/00028312041001159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shillcock, R. (1982). The on-line resolution of pronominal anaphora. Language and Speech, 25, 385–402.

    Google Scholar 

  • Speelman, C. P., & Kirsner, K. (1990). The representation of text-based and situation-based information in discourse comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 119–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strunk, W., & White, E. B. (2000). The elements of style (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swales, J. (2005). Attended and unattended “this” in academic writing: A long and unfinished story. ESP Malaysia, 11, 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swales, J. M., & Feak, C. B. (2000). English in today’s research world: A writing guide. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Swales, J. M., & Feak, C. B. (2004). Academic writing for graduate students (2nd ed.). Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webber, B. L. (1991). Structure and ostension in the interpretation of discourse deixis. Language and Cognitive Processes, 6, 107–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. M. (1985). Style: Ten lessons in clarity and grace (2nd ed.). Glenview, IL: Scott Foresman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witte, S. P., & Faigley, L. (1981). Coherence, cohesion, and writing quality. College Composition and Communication, 32(2), 189–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Witten, I. A., Frank, E., & Hall, M. A. (2011). Data mining. San Francisco, CA: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wulff, S., Römer, U., & Swale, J. (2012). Attended/unattended this in academic student writing: Quantitative and qualitative perspectives. Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 8(1), 129–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yang, W. W., Lu, X., & Weigle, S. C. (2015). Different topics, different discourse: Relationships among writing topic, measures of syntactic complexity, and judgments of writing quality. Journal of Second Language Writing, 28, 53–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported in part by the Institute for Education Sciences (IES R305A080589 and IES R305G20018-02). Ideas expressed in this material are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the IES. We thank our expert raters for their assistance in scoring the essays used in this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Scott A. Crossley.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Crossley, S.A., Rose, D.F., Danekes, C. et al. That noun phrase may be beneficial and this may not be: discourse cohesion in reading and writing. Read Writ 30, 569–589 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9690-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9690-4

Keywords

Navigation