Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Sourcing in professional education: Do text factors make any difference?

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study investigated the extent to which the text factors of source salience and emphasis on risk might influence readers’ attention to and use of source information when reading single documents to make behavioral decisions on controversial health-related issues. Participants (n = 259), who were attending different bachelor-level professional programs at a university college, generally disregarded source information irrespective of textual manipulations, especially sources cited or embedded within other documents, and mainly relied on their own personal experiences and opinions when making behavioral decisions on the issues. Theoretical as well as educational implications of the findings are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In this article, the term primary sources is not used in the conventional sense, that is, as documents written by persons directly involved in (historical) events, but denotes metadata about a particular document (e.g., about its author or date of publication) that may or may not cite other sources (termed embedded sources in this article) within its boundaries.

  2. All 259 students reported daily use of cell phones. When reporting on their weekly intake of artificial sweeteners, 150 students reported drinking more than .5 l a week and the rest reported drinking 0–.5 l a week. It is thus possible that some participants falling in the latter category may not have consumed any artificial sweeteners at all.

  3. While it might be argued that the attribution task underestimated participants’ sourcing because they assumed it was sufficient to report on only one type of source features (primary or embedded) although they had paid attention to and remembered both types, this seems unlikely because the scores for both primary and embedded source features were very similar on the attribution and the cued recall tasks. That is, if there had been a systematic underestimation of source feature scores on the attribution task because participants were not explicitly asked to provide information about both types of sources on this task, one would have expected considerably lower scores on this task than on the cued recall task. Moreover, the possibility of a systematic underestimation of scores on the attribution task in this study, implies that the “true” scores on this task were likely considerably higher than on the cued recall task, which does not seem to be a reasonable assumption given that the latter not only explicitly asked participants to report everything they remembered about primary as well as embedded sources but also cued their recall of each type of source by explicit mentioning of five distinct source features. Please also see the description of the attribution task in the Method section.

  4. Please note that the fact that students quite seldom referred to document content when asked to justifiy their behavioral decisions does not preclude their prioritizing of content over source information during reading. This is because their references to their own experiences and opinions rather than document content in their justifications may suggest that they regarded those reasons as more important than content, which does not necessarily imply that they disregarded content during reading.

References

  • Alexander, P. A., & the Diciplined Reading and Learning Research Laboratory. (2012). Reading into the future: Competence for the 21st century. Educational Psychologist, 47, 259–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andiliou, A., Ramsay, C. M., Murphy, P. K., & Fast, J. (2012). Weighing opposing positions: Examining the effects of intratextual persuasive messages on students’ knowledge and beliefs. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 37, 113–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andreassen, R., & Bråten, I. (2013). Teachers’ source evaluation self-efficacy predicts their use of relevant source features when evaluating the trustworthiness of web sources on special education. British Journal of Educational Technology, 44, 821–836.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anmarkrud, Ø., Bråten, I., & Strømsø, H. I. (2014). Multiple-documents literacy: Strategic processing, source awareness, and argumentation when reading multiple conflicting documents. Learning and Individual Differences, 30, 64–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (1990). Human memory: Theory and practice. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bazerman, C. (1985). Physicists reading physics: Schema-laden purposes and purpose-laden schema. Written Communication, 2, 3–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Björnsson, C. H. (1968). Läsbarhet [Readability]. Stockholm: Liber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Björnsson, C. H. (1983). Readability of newspapers in 11 languages. Reading Research Quarterly, 18, 480–497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braasch, J. L. G., Bråten, I., Strømsø, H. I., Anmarkrud, Ø., & Ferguson, L. E. (2013). Promoting secondary school students’ evaluation of source features of multiple documents. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38, 180–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braasch, J. L. G., Rouet, J. F., Vibert, N., & Britt, M. A. (2012). Readers’ use of source information in text comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 40, 450–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brand-Gruwel, S., & Stadtler, M. (2011). Solving information-based problems: Evaluating sources of information. Learning and Instruction, 21, 175–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bråten, I., Britt, M. A., Strømsø, H. I., & Rouet, J. F. (2011). The role of epistemic beliefs in the comprehension of multiple expository texts: Toward an integrated model. Educational Psychologist, 46, 48–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bråten, I., Salmerón, L., & Strømsø, H. I. (2015). Who said that? Investigating the Plausibility-Induced Source Focusing assumption with Norwegian undergraduate readers. Submitted manuscript.

  • Bråten, I., Strømsø, H. I., & Britt, M. A. (2009). Trust matters: Examining the role of source evaluation in students’ construction of meaning within and across multiple texts. Reading Research Quarterly, 44, 6–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bråten, I., Strømsø, H. I., & Salmerón, L. (2011). Trust and mistrust when students read multiple information sources about climate change. Learning and Instruction, 21, 180–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Britt, M. A., & Aglinskas, C. (2002). Improving students’ ability to identify and use source information. Cognition and Instruction, 20, 485–522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Britt, M. A., & Gabrys, G. (2002). Implications of document-level literacy skills for web-site design. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 34, 170–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Britt, M. A., Perfetti, C. A., Sandak, R., & Rouet, J. F. (1999). Content integration and source separation in learning from multiple texts. In S. R. Goldman, A. C. Graesser, & P. van den Broek (Eds.), Narrative, comprehension, causality, and coherence: Essays in honor of Tom Trabasso (pp. 209–233). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Britt, M. A., & Rouet, J. F. (2012). Learning with multiple documents: Component skills and their acquisition. In J. R. Kirby & M. J. Lawson (Eds.), Enhancing the quality of learning: Dispositions, instruction, and learning processes (pp. 276–314). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Britt, M. A., Rouet, J. F., & Braasch, J. L. G. (2013). Documents experienced as entities: Extending the situation model theory of comprehension. In M. A. Britt, S. R. Goldman, & J.-F. Rouet (Eds.), Reading from words to multiple texts (pp. 160–179). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Pereyra, G., Britt, M. A., Braasch, J. L. G., & Rouet, J. F. (2014). Readers’ memory for information sources in simple news stories: Effects of text and task features. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26, 187–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Der Spiegel (2011). Merkel has disgraced herself. Retrieved 02.03.11 from Spiegel Online: http://www.spiegel.de/international/germany/0,1518,748624,00.html.

  • Dunlosky, J., & Metcalfe, J. (2009). Metacognition. Los Angeles: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenbach, G. (2008). Credibility of health information and digital media: New perspectives and implications for youth. In M. J. Metzger & A. J. Flanagin (Eds.), Digital media, youth, and credibility (pp. 123–154). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Field, A. (2010). Discovering statistics using SPSS (3rd ed.). Los Angeles: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerjets, P., Kammerer, Y., & Werner, B. (2011). Measuring spontaneous and instructed evaluation processes during web search: Integrating concurrent thinking-aloud protocols and eye-tracking data. Learning and Instruction, 21, 220–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, S. R., & Bloome, D. M. (2004). Learning to construct and integrate. In A. F. Healy (Ed.), Experimental cognitive psychology and its applications: Festschrift in honor of Lyle Bourne, Walter Kintsch, and Thomas Landauer (pp. 169–182). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, S. R., Braasch, J. L. G., Wiley, J., Graesser, A. C., & Brodowinska, K. (2012). Comprehending and learning from Internet sources: Processing patterns of better and poorer learners. Reading Research Quarterly, 47, 356–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A. C., Bowers, C., Olde, B., & Pomeroy, V. (1999). Who said what? Source memory for narrator and character agents in literary short stories. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 284–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A. C., & McNamara, D. S. (2011). Computational analyses of multilevel discourse comprehension. Topics in Cognitive Science, 3, 371–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, J. A., & Johnson, M. K. (2012). Some thoughts on the interaction between perception and reflection. In J. M. Wolfe & L. Robertson (Eds.), From perception to consciousness: Searching with Anne Treisman (pp. 390–397). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. K., & Hirst, W. (1993). MEM: Memory subsystems as processes. In A. F. Collins, S. E. Gathercole, M. A. Conway, & P. E. Morris (Eds.), Theories of memory (pp. 241–286). Sussex: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jungermann, H., Pfister, H. R., & Fischer, K. (1996). Credibility, information preferences, and information interests. Risk Analysis, 16, 251–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, J., & Millis, K. (2006). The influence of sourcing and relatedness on event integration. Discourse Processes, 41, 51–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95, 163–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kolstø, S. D. (2001). “To trust or not to trust, …”—pupils’ ways of judging information encountered in a socio-scientific issue. International Journal of Science Education, 23, 877–901.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leu, D. J., Kinzer, C. K., Coiro, J., Castek, J., & Henry, L. A. (2013). New literacies: A dual-level theory of the changing nature of literacy, instruction, and assessment. In D. E. Alvermann, N. J. Unrau, & R. B. Ruddell (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading (6th ed., pp. 1150–1181). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Linn, M. C., & Eylon, B.-S. (2006). Science education: Integrating views of learning and instruction. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 511–544). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lundeberg, M. A. (1987). Metacognitive aspects of reading comprehension: Studying understanding in legal case analysis. Reading Research Quarterly, 22, 407–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macedo-Rouet, M., Braasch, J. L. G., Britt, M. A., & Rouet, J. F. (2013). Teaching fourth and fifth graders to evaluate information sources during text comprehension. Cognition and Instruction, 31, 204–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maggioni, L., & Fox, E. (2009). Adolescents’ reading of multiple history texts: An interdisciplinary investigation of historical thinking, intertextual reading, and domain-specific epistemic beliefs. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Diego, CA.

  • Maier, J., & Richter, T. (2013). Text-belief consistency effects in the comprehension of multiple texts with conflicting information. Cognition and Instruction, 31, 151–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, L., Junyent, A. A., & Tornatora, M. C. (2014). Epistemic evaluation and comprehension of web-source information on controversial science-related topics: Effects of a short-term instructional intervention. Computers & Education, 76, 143–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nokes, J., Dole, J., & Hacker, D. J. (2007). Teaching high school students to be critical and strategic readers of historical texts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 492–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norris, S. P., Phillips, L. M., & Korpan, C. A. (2003). University students’ interpretation of media reports of science and its relationship to background knowledge, interest, and reading difficulty. Public Understanding of Science, 12, 123–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norwegian Ministry of Education and Research. (2006). Læreplan for grunnskolen og videregående skole [Curriculum for elementary and secondary school]. Oslo: Ministry of Education and Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Britt, M. A., & Georgi, M. C. (1995). Text-based learning and reasoning: Studies in history. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Rouet, J. F., & Britt, M. A. (1999). Toward a theory of documents representation. In H. Van Oostendorp & S. R. Goldman (Eds.), The construction of mental representation during reading (pp. 99–122). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2012). The Elaboration Likelihood Model. In P. A. M. Van Lange, A. Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 224–245). London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., & Wegener, D. T. (1999). The Elaboration Likelihood Model: Current status and controversies. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 41–72). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, L. M., & Norris, S. P. (1999). Interpreting popular reports of science: What happens when the reader’s world meets the world on paper? International Journal of Science Education, 21, 317–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pornpitakpan, C. (2004). The persuasiveness of source credibility: A critical review of five decades’ evidence. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34, 243–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M., & Afflerbach, P. (1995). Verbal protocols of reading: The nature of constructively responsive reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reisman, A. (2012). A document-based history curriculum intervention in urban high schools. Cognition and Instruction, 30, 86–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richter, T., & Rapp, D. N. (2014). Comprehension and validation of text information. Discourse Processes, 51, 1–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rouet, J. F. (2006). The skills of document use: From text comprehension to Web-based learning. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rouet, J.-F., Britt, M. A., Mason, R. A., & Perfetti, C. A. (1996). Using multiple sources of evidence to reason about history. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 478–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanchez, C. A., Wiley, J., & Goldman, S. R. (2006). Teaching students to evaluate source reliability during Internet research tasks. In S. A. Barab, K. E. Hay, & D. T. Hickey (Eds.), Proceedings of the seventh international conference on the learning sciences (pp. 662–666). Bloomington, IN: International Society of the Learning Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shanahan, C., Shanahan, T., & Misischia, C. (2011). Analysis of expert readers in three disciplines: History, mathematics, and chemistry. Journal of Literacy Research, 43, 393–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, J. R., & Rapp, D. N. (2011). Readers’ reliance on source credibility in the service of comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 230–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stadtler, M., & Bromme, R. (2007). Dealing with multiple documents on the WWW: The role of metacognition in the formation of documents models. International Journal of Computer Supported Collaborative Learning, 2, 191–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stadtler, M., & Bromme, R. (2013). Multiple document comprehension: An approach to public understanding of science. Cognition and Instruction, 31, 122–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stadtler, M., Bromme, R., & Rouet, J. F. (2014). “Science meets reading”: Worin bestehen die Kompetenzen zum Lesen multipler Dokumente zu Wissenschaftsthemen und wie fördert man sie (What are the competencies of reading multiple documents on scientific topics and how can they be taught)? Unterrichtswissenschaft, 42, 55–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stadtler, M., Scharrer, L., Brummernhenrich, B., & Bromme, R. (2013a). Dealing with uncertainty: Readers’ memory for and use of conflicting information from science texts as a function of presentation format and source expertise. Cognition and Instruction, 31, 130–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stadtler, M., Thomm, E., Babiel, S., Hentschke, J., & Bromme, R. (2013). Ignorant albeit competent: Examining students’ sourcing competencies and spontaneous use of source information while reading conflicting scientific texts. Paper presented at the Workshop on Multiple Document Literacy, Münster, Germany.

  • Stahl, S. A., Hynd, C. R., Britton, B. K., McNish, M. M., & Bosquet, D. (1996). What happens when students read multiple source documents in history? Reading Research Quarterly, 31, 430–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2008). On the relative independence of thinking biases and cognitive ability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 672–695.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steffens, B., Britt, M. A., Braasch, J. L. G., Strømsø, H. I., & Bråten, I. (2014). Memory for scientific arguments and their sources: Claim-evidence consistency matters. Discourse Processes, 51, 117–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strømsø, H. I., & Bråten, I. (2014). Students’ sourcing while reading and writing from multiple web documents. Nordic Journal of Digital Literacy, 9, 92–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strømsø, H. I., Bråten, I., & Britt, M. A. (2010). Reading multiple texts about climate change: The relationship between memory for sources and text comprehension. Learning and Instruction, 20, 192–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strømsø, H. I., Bråten, I., Britt, M. A., & Ferguson, L. E. (2013). Spontaneous sourcing among students reading multiple documents. Cognition and Instruction, 31, 176–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dijk, T. A. (1999). Context models in discourse processing. In H. van Oostendorp & S. R. Goldman (Eds.), The construction of mental representations during reading (pp. 123–148). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Strien, J. L. H., Brand-Gruwel, S., & Boshuizen, H. P. A. (2014). Dealing with conflicting information from multiple nonlinear texts: Effects of prior attitudes. Computers in Human Behavior, 32, 101–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • VanSledright, B., & Kelly, C. (1998). Reading American history: The influence on multiple sources on six fifth graders. The Elementary School Journal, 98, 239–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vinje, F. E. (1982). Journalistspråket [The journalist language]. Fredrikstad: Institute for Journalism.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walraven, A., Brand-Gruwel, S., & Boshuizen, H. P. A. (2013). Fostering students’ evaluation behavior while searching the Internet. Instructional Science, 41, 125–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, K., & Mejia-Ramos, J. P. (2013). The influence of sources in the reading of mathematical text: A reply to Shanahan, Shanahan, and Misischia. Journal of Literacy Research, 45, 87–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiley, J., Goldman, S. R., Graesser, A. C., Sanchez, C. A., Ash, I. K., & Hemmerich, J. A. (2009). Source evaluation, comprehension, and learning in Internet science inquiry tasks. American Educational Research Journal, 46, 1060–1106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wineburg, S. S. (1991). Historical problem solving: A study of the cognitive processes used in the evaluation of documentary and pictorial evidence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 73–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wyatt, D., Pressley, M., El-Dinary, P. B., Stein, S., Evans, P., & Brown, R. (1993). Comprehension strategies, worth and credibility monitoring, and evaluations: Cold and hot cognition when experts read professional articles that are important to them. Learning and Individual Differences., 5, 49–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, F. Y., & Tsai, C. C. (2010). An epistemic framework for scientific reasoning in informal contexts. In L. D. Bendixen & F. C. Feucht (Eds.), Personal epistemology in the classroom (pp. 124–162). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ivar Bråten.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bråten, I., Strømsø, H.I. & Andreassen, R. Sourcing in professional education: Do text factors make any difference?. Read Writ 29, 1599–1628 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9611-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9611-y

Keywords

Navigation