Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Effectiveness of word solving: Integrating morphological problem-solving within comprehension instruction for middle school students

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study explores the effectiveness of integrating morphological instruction within comprehension strategy instruction. Participants were 203 students (N = 117 fifth-grade; 86 sixth-grade) from four urban schools who were randomly assigned to the intervention (N = 110; morphological problem-solving within comprehension strategy instruction) or comparison condition (N = 90; comprehension strategy instruction). All students received four thirty-minute small-group guided reading sessions involving comprehension strategy instruction with students in the intervention also learning about morphological problem-solving. Results suggested the intervention versus comparison instruction was moderately more effective at supporting multiple choice and self-perceived vocabulary knowledge (g = 0.41, g = 0.47) and also morphological awareness via generation of morphologically related words (per word g = 0.51; total g = 0.69). No significant differences were noted for other literacy outcomes such as reading comprehension and word reading fluency. Interactions between intervention status and pretest scores for self-perceived vocabulary knowledge and generation of morphologically related words indicated that while the intervention was effective for all, it was particularly helpful for readers with lower pretest scores. An interaction between language background and total number of morphologically related words generated suggested that the intervention was particularly supportive of language minority youth. Implications are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anglin, J. M. (1993). Vocabulary development: A morphological analysis. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58, 1–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • August, D., & Shanahan, T. (2006). Developing literacy in second-language learners. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • August, D., & Shanahan, T. (2010). Response to a review and update on developing literacy in second language learners: Report of the national literacy panel on language minority children and youth. Journal of Literacy Research, 42, 341–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumann, J. F., Edwards, E. C., Boland, E. M., Olejnik, S. F., & Kame’enui, E. J. (2003). Vocabulary tricks: Effects of instruction in morphology and context on fifth-grade students’ ability to derive and infer word meanings. American Educational Research Journal, 40(2), 447–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biemiller, A. (2005). Size and sequence in vocabulary development: Implications for choosing words for primary grade vocabulary instruction. In E. H. Hiebert & M. L. Kamil (Eds.), Teaching and learning vocabulary: Bringing research to practice (pp. 223–242). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. N., Kirby, J. R., & Deacon, S. H. (2010). The effects of morphological instruction on literacy skills: A systematic review of the literature. Review of Educational Research, 80, 144–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F. (2010). Effects of instruction in morphological awareness on literacy achievement: An integrative review. Reading Research Quarterly, 45(4), 464–487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlo, M. S., August, D., Mclaughlin, B., Snow, C. E., Dressler, C., Lippman, D. N., et al. (2004). Closing the gap: Addressing the vocabulary needs of English-language learners in bilingual and mainstream classrooms. Reading Research Quarterly, 39(2), 188–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20, 37–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coxhead, A. (2000). A new academic word list. TESOL Quarterly, 34, 213–238. doi:10.2307/3587951.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flanigan, K., Hayes, L., Templeton, S., Bear, D. W., Invernizzi, M., & Johnston, F. (2011). Words their way with struggling readers: Word study for reading, vocabulary, and spelling instruction, Grades 4-12. Boston: Pearson/Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fritz, C. O., Morris, P. E., & Richler, J. J. (2012). Effect size estimates: Current use, calculations, and interpretation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 141(1), 2–18. doi:10.1037/a0024338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R. (2012). Towards a universal model of reading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 263–329. doi:10.1017/S0140525X11001841.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, L. S., & Fuchs, D. (1992). Identifying a measure for monitoring student reading progress. School Psychology Review, 21(1), 45–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giovanni, N., & Collier, B. (2006). Rosa. New York: Square Fish.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, A. P., & Ahn, S. (2010). A meta-analysis of morphological interventions: Effects on literacy achievement of children with literacy difficulties. Annals of dyslexia, 60(2), 183–208. doi:10.1007/s11881-010-0041-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, A. P., & Ahn, S. (2013). A meta-analysis of morphological interventions in English: Effects on literacy outcomes for school-age children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(4), 257–285. doi:10.1080/10888438.2012.689791.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, A. P., & Perkins, J. (2015). Word detectives: Morphological instruction that supports academic language. The Reading Teacher, 68(7), 504–517. doi:10.1002/trtr.1342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, A. P., Gilbert, J. K., Cho, S. J., & Kearns, D. M. (2014). Probing lexical representations: Simultaneous modeling of word and person contributions to multidimensional lexical representations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(2), 448–468. doi:10.1037/a0034754.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, A. P., Petscher, Y., Carlisle, J. F., & Mitchell, A. M. (2015). Exploring the dimensionality of morphological knowledge for adolescent readers. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Hedges, L. V. (1981). Distribution theory for Glass’s estimator of effect size and related estimators. Journal of Educational Statistics, 6, 107–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, C. J., Bloom, H. S., Rebeck Black, A., & Lipsey, M. W. (2008). Empirical benchmarks for interpreting effect sizes in research. Child Development Perspectives, 2(3), 172–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • IBM Corp. (2012). IBM SPSS statistics for windows, Version 21.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.

  • Lesaux, N. K., & Kieffer, M. J. (2010). Exploring sources of reading comprehension difficulties among language minority learners and their classmates in early adolescence. American Educational Research Journal, 47(3), 596–632.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lesaux, N. K., Kieffer, M. J., Kelley, J. G., & Harris, J. R. (2014). Effects of academic vocabulary instruction for linguistically diverse adolescents: Evidence from a randomized field trial. American Educational Research Journal. doi:10.3102/0002831214532165.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacGinitie, W., MacGinitie, R., Maria, K., & Dreyer, L. G. (2000). Gates-MacGinitie reading test (4th ed.). Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mahony, D. L. (1994). Using sensitivity to word structure to explain variance in high school and college level reading ability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 6, 19–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mather, N., Hammill, D. D., Allen, E. A., & Roberts, R. (2004). Test of silent word reading fluency. Texas: PRO-ED.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCutchen, D., Logan, B., & Biangardi-Orpe, U. (2009). Making meaning: Children’s sensitivity to morphological information during word reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 44(4), 360–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W., & Anderson, R. (1984). The number of words in printed school English. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 304–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W. E., Garcia, G. E., Durgunoglu, A. Y., & Hancin-Bhatt, B. (1993). Spanish-English bilingual students’ use of cognates in English reading. Journal of Reading Behavior, 25(3), 241–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W. E., & Townsend, D. (2012). Words as tools: Learning academic vocabulary as language acquisition. Reading Research Quarterly, 47(1), 91–108. doi:10.1002/RRQ.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pacheco, M. B., & Goodwin, A. P. (2013). Putting two and two together: Middle school students’ morphological problem solving strategies for unknown words. Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy, 56(7), 541–553. doi:10.1002/JAAL.181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 357–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational tools for probing interaction effects in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 31, 437–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raudenbush, S. W., Martinez, A., & Spybrook, J. (2007). Strategies for improving precision in group-randomized experiments. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 9, 5–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reichle, E. D., & Perfetti, C. A. (2003). Morphology in word identification: A word experience model that accounts for morpheme frequency effects. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7(3), 219–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ride, S., & Okie, S. (1986). To space and back. New York: HarperCollins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schreuder, R., & Baayan, R. H. (1995). Modeling morphological processing. In L. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 131–154). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shanahan, T., Callison, K., Carriere, C., Duke, N. K., Pearson, P. D., Schatschneider, C., et al. (2010). Improving reading comprehension in kindergarten through 3rd grade: A practice guide (NCEE 2010-4038). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Retrieved from http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/pdf/practiceguides/readingcomp_pg_092810.pdf

  • Singson, M., Mahony, D., & Mann, V. (2000). The relation between reading ability and morphological skills: Evidence from derivational suffixes. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 219–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, A., & Nagy, W. (1989). The acquisition of English derivational morphology. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 649–667.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, A., & Nagy, W. E. (1990). Use of derivational morphology during reading. Cognition, 36, 17–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J., Rashotte, C. A., & Pearson, N. (2010). Test of sentence reading efficiency and comprehension. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • What Works Clearinghouse. (2008). WWC procedures and standards handbook. Retrieved from http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/pdf/reference_resources/wwc_procedures_v2_1_standards_handbook.pdf

  • Woodcock, R. W., & Johnson, M. B. (1990). Woodcock-Johnson psycho-educational battery-revised tests of achievement. Itasca, IL: Riverside.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeno, S. M., Ivens, S. H., Millard, R. T., & Duvvuri, R. (1995). The educator’s word frequency guide. New York: Touchstone Applied Science Associates.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was made possible through funding from Vanderbilt University’s Peabody College. I would like to thank the research team that supported the project and the students and teachers who participated in the study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Amanda P. Goodwin.

Appendix: Details about intervention and comparison instruction

Appendix: Details about intervention and comparison instruction

Instruction occurred in small groups (2–7 students) following a guided reading format that involved four 30 min instructional sessions. All instruction was scripted, although tutors were encouraged to adapt the instruction based on the needs of their students and time constraints as long as they completed the main components of the intervention. The main components were identified as (1) tutors teaching students about comprehension strategies, (2) students reading and coding use of strategies in the chosen texts using flags or sticky notes, (3) tutors coding main points (i.e., clues from the text) on the white board, (4) students and tutors either word solving (i.e., morphological problem-solving) in the intervention or defining words in the comparison condition, (5) tutors including closure to the lesson, and (6) students playing the suggested reinforcement game.

All students received comprehension instruction as part of both the intervention and comparison condition. This instruction involved research-based strategies (i.e., comprehension strategy instruction as suggested within Shanahan et al., 2010) and fit within the guidelines of the Common Core Standards (National Governors Association Center for Best Practices & Council of Chief State School, 2010). Standards that were addressed included having students cite evidence, determine the meanings of words within text, read and understand grade-level texts, and learn and use grade appropriate academic vocabulary. These standards were developed by teaching students to flag use of comprehension strategies (i.e., visualizing and making connections) while reading two content specific texts: an informational Science text To Space and Back (Ride & Okie, 1986) and a Social Studies narrative text, Rosa (Giovanni & Collier, 2006). Two sessions were spent on each text.

General components of good instruction were also prioritized such as opportunities for shared and independent reading, use of the guided-release model (i.e., tutors modeled strategies, then worked with students in using the strategies together, and then supported independent use of the strategies), a focus on motivation (i.e., we asked students to search for clues to solve cases and awarded points for clues shared), use of visuals (i.e., tutors and students coded clues from text on a whiteboard, which we called a clue chart), use of isolated word instruction & word instruction integrated within text, use of games to reinforce learning and build engagement, and a focus on transfer across subject areas. An example comparison and intervention script is available from the author. For more information, see Goodwin and Perkins (2015), which details the intervention for educators.

The intervention and comparison conditions differed because the intervention condition included morphological instruction. Specifically, students were taught to use ‘word solving’ (i.e., morphological problem-solving) as an additional reading comprehension strategy. For comparison students, challenging words were pre-taught or defined and discussed with examples and non-examples, while in the intervention condition, students word solved unfamiliar words within text by finding units of meaning they recognized within large, unfamiliar words. Those morphemes were marked (i.e., boxed or circled) and then each separate unit was defined. Next, students were encouraged to sum the meanings of the parts (i.e., put the meanings together) and then link to story context to confirm whether the new meaning made sense within the story. Tutors modeled this strategy. Then students enacted the strategy within the text, representing their word solving on sticky notes and on the white board. Sessions 1 and 2 focused on using smaller units to figure out the meaning of unfamiliar larger words, and session 3 added in the strategy of using knowledge of a larger word (i.e., astronaut = someone in space) to figure out the meaning of smaller units in other words (i.e., astro = space, so astronomy and astronomer must have something to do with space). Session 4 involved practicing both morphological problem-solving strategies, as well as the other comprehension strategies that were part of both the intervention and comparison instruction.

The other main difference between the intervention and comparison condition was the games that were used to reinforce learning. In the intervention, the games were morphological in nature. The first, Word Webbing presented students with a unit of meaning like move and asked them to write down as many morphologically related words as possible in a minute like movement, movers, remove, removable, or unmovable. The second, Find the Imposter, involved presenting students with a target word like effortlessly and then asking the student to circle all related words (i.e., words that share a root or affix like effortful, efforts, fearless) and to cross out any imposters (i.e., words that overlapped orthography, but did not share morphemes like effects or fort). The comparison students played a game Zoom where students answered questions related to the text read that allowed them to practice their comprehension strategies (i.e., students were given a quote from the text and then asked to list three things that they saw within that scene in order to practice visualizing). Students moved ahead the allotted number of spaces for each correct answer.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Goodwin, A.P. Effectiveness of word solving: Integrating morphological problem-solving within comprehension instruction for middle school students. Read Writ 29, 91–116 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9581-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9581-0

Keywords

Navigation