Skip to main content
Log in

Word and nonword processing without meaning support in Korean-speaking children with and without hyperlexia

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Hyperlexia is a syndrome of reading without meaning in individuals who otherwise have pronounced cognitive and language deficits. The present study investigated the quality of word representation and the effects of deficient semantic processing on word and nonword reading of Korean children with hyperlexia; their performances were compared to those of typical children matched for their word-recognition abilities. In Study 1, both groups of children were tested on their ability to read word-like nonwords (nonsense words that are generated from real words). Typical children showed slower read-aloud times when reading word-like nonwords compared to real words; children with hyperlexia showed no significant differences between the two types of words. These results suggest that typical children were disrupted by information from the baseword (e.g., ‘self’) on which the nonword is based (e.g., ‘melf’) when reading word-like nonwords, whereas children with hyperlexia were not interrupted by such information. In Study 2, the same two groups were compared on their word reading performances between semantically related picture-word versus semantically unrelated picture-word pairs. Typical children read more slowly the words preceded by semantically related pictures than the words preceded by semantically unrelated pictures; children with hyperlexia read both picture-word pairs at a comparable rate. These results imply that, during word reading, typical children were affected by semantic information from the preceding picture unlike children with hyperlexia who were not influenced by the preceding semantic information. These findings from Study 1 and Study 2 are discussed in relation to the connectionist models of reading.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The nonword가기/kagi/can be used as a real word in some contexts (e.g., 바로 가기). Therefore, the analyses were repeated after excluding the nonword 가기. The same results were obtained when the analysis was repeated on the mean error rate. Almost identical results were obtained when the analysis was repeated on the reading time (a significant main effect of WordType, F(1,18) = 7.63, p = .013, η 2p  = .30; a significant WordType-Group interaction, F(1, 18) = 7.52, p = .013, η 2p  = .30); non-significant main effect of Group, F(1, 18) = 3.61, p = .073). Post-hoc test results also revealed the same patterns: while the children without hyperlexia read words significantly faster than nonwords (t(9) = −3.185, p = .011, d = −1.04), the children with hyperlexia did not (t(9) = .020, p = .984).

References

  • Adams, M. J. (2000). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: The Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aram, D. M. (1997). Hyperlexia: Reading without meaning in young children. Topics in Language Disorders, 17, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aram, D. M., & Healy, J. M. (1988). Hyperlexia: A review of extraordinary word recognition. In L. K. Obler & D. Fein (Eds.), The exceptional brain: Neuropsychology of talent and special abilities (pp. 70–102). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aram, D. M., Rose, D. F., & Horwitz, S. J. (1984). Hyperlexia: Developmental reading without meaning. In R. Malatesha & H. A. Whitaker (Eds.), Dyslexia: A global issue (pp. 517–531). The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, C. A. (1980). Semantic context effects in visual word recognition: An analysis of semantic strategies. Memory & Cognition, 8, 493–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burd, L., Kerbeshian, J., & Fisher, W. (1985). Inquiry into the incidence of hyperlexia in a statewide population of children with pervasive developmental disorder. Psychological Reports, 57, 236–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cardoso-Martins, C., & Silva, J. R. (2010). Cognitive and language correlates of hyperlexia: Evidence from children with autism spectrum disorders. Reading and Writing, 23, 129–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 47, 149–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H., Hogan, T. P., & Fey, M. (2003). Subgrouping poor readers on the basis of reading-related abilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 151–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Choi, H. P., Kwak, K., & Park, K. (1996). Korean-Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence. Seoul: Special education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, M. J., Hall, J., & Riccio, C. A. (1997). Neuropsychological profiles of children diagnosed as specific language impaired with and without hyperlexia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 12, 223–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Strategies of Information Processing (pp.151–216). London: Academic Press.

  • Coltheart, M. (2006). Dual route and connectionist models of reading: An overview. London Review of Education, 4(1), 5–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K. L., & Forster, J. C. (n. d.). DMDX display software. Retrieved from http://www.u.arizona.edu/~kforster/dmdx/dmdx.htm.

  • Glosser, G., Friedman, R. B., & Roeltgen, D. P. (1996). Clues to the cognitive organization of reading and writing from developmental hyperlexia. Neuropsychology, 10, 168–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, T. E., & Rothermel, R. D. (1984). Hyperlexic children reading. Brain, 107, 759–785.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Healy, J. M. (1982). The enigma of hyperlexia. Reading Research Quarterly, 17, 319–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Healy, J. M., Aram, D. M., Horwitz, S. J., & Kessler, J. W. (1982). A study of hyperlexia. Brain and Language, 17, 1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutchison, K. A. (2003). Is semantic priming due to association strength or featural overlap? A micro-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 785–813.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joshi, R. M., Padakannaya, P., & Nishanimath, R. (2010). Dyslexia and hyperlexia in bilinguals. Dyslexia, 16, 99–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, B. (2003). Hyperlexia profiles. Brain and Language, 84(2), 204–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, J., & Davis, C. (2004). Characteristics of poor readers of Korean Hangul: Auditory, visual, and phonological processing. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17, 153–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y. T., Seong, T. J., & Lee, Y. K. (2003). Preschool Receptive-Expressive Language Scale (PRES). Seoul: Seoul Community Rehabilitation Center.

    Google Scholar 

  • Korean Educational Development Institute. (1989). KEDI-Individual Basic Learning Skills Test (KEDI). Seoul: Korean Educational Development Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laxon, V. J., Coltheart, V., & Keating, C. (1988). Children find friendly words friendly too: Words with many orthographic neighbors are easier to read and spell. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 58, 103–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, M. (2000). Semantic priming without association: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 618–630.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of the effect of context in perception, Part I. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, D. E., Schvaneveldt, R. W., & Ruddy, M. G. (1975). Loci of contextual effects on visual word recognition. In S. Dornic (Ed.), Attention and Performance VI (Vol. 5, pp. 98–118). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, K., McKone, E., & Slee, J. (2003). Dissociations between implicit and explicit memory in children: The role of strategic processing and the knowledge base. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84, 124–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nam, M., & Park, R. (2002).Comparison between pervasive developmental disorder and developmental delayed children with hyperlexia. Paper session presented at the Ninth Conference of Korean Association for Persons with Autism, Seoul, Korea.

  • Nation, K. (1999). Reading skills in hyperlexia: A developmental perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 338–355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Snowling, M. (1998). Contextual facilitation of word recognition: Evidence from dyslexia and poor reading comprehension. Child Development, 69, 996–1011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (1999). Developmental differences in sensitivity to semantic relations among good and poor comprehenders: Evidence from semantic priming. Cognition, 70, B1–B13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, T. M., Macomber, D., Babitz, T., Volkmar, F., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2007). Hyperlexia in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 760–774.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nippold, M. A. (2002). Lexical learning in school-age children, adolescents, and adults: a process where language and literacy converge. Journal of Child Language, 29(02), 449–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Park, E., Hwang, M., & Nam, M. (2003). The reading skills and language and cognitive characteristics of children with hyperlexia. Korean Journal of Communication Disorders, 8(1), 82–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patti, P. J., & Lupinetti, L. (1993). Brief report: Implications of hyperlexia in an autistic savant. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23, 397–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. P., Johnson, C., & Welsh, M. C. (1987). Unexpected reading precocity in a normal preschooler: Implications for hyperlexia. Brain and Language, 30, 165–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaut, D. C. (1999). A connectionist approach to word reading and acquired dyslexia: Extension to sequential processing. Cognitive Science, 23, 543–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J. L., Seidenberg, M. S., & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review, 103, 56–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richman, L. C., & Kitchell, M. M. (1981). Hyperlexia as a variant of developmental language disorder. Brain and Language, 12, 203–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saldaña, D., Carreiras, M., & Frith, U. (2009). Orthographic and phonological pathways in hyperlexic readers with autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34(3), 240–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., Chiu, C. Y. P., & Ochsner, K. N. (1993). Implicit memory: A selective review. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 16, 156–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M. F., Marin, O. S. M., & Saffran, E. M. (1979). Dissociations of language function in dementia: A case study. Brain and Language, 7, 277–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M. F., Saffran, E. M., & Marin, O. S. M. (1980). Fractionating the reading process: Evidence for word-specific, print-to-sound associations. In M. Coltheart, K. Patterson, & J. C. Marshall (Eds.), Deep dyslexia (pp. 259–269). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S. (2007). Connectionist models of reading. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 238–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S., & Plaut, D. C. (1998). Evaluating word reading models at the item level: Matching the grain of theory and data. Psychological Science, 9, 234–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S. (1984). A longitudinal study of a hyperlexic child: Hyperlexia as a language disorder. Neuropsychologia, 22, 557–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silberberg, N., & Silberberg, M. C. (1971). Hyperlexia: The other end of the continuum. Journal of Special Education, 5, 233–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R. L. (2001). Phonemic awareness and reading skills in hyperlexic children: A longitudinal study. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 333–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., & Powell, J. S. (1983). Comprehending verbal comprehension. American Psychologist, 38, 878–893.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stothard, S. E., Snowling, M. I., Bishop, D. V. M., Chipchase, B. B., & Kaplan, C. A. (1998). Language impaired pre-schoolers: A follow-up into adolescence. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 41, 407–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Goswami, U., & Bruck, M. (1990). Not all nonwords are alike: Implications for reading development and theory. Memory and Cognition, 18, 559–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E., & Schacter, D. L. (1990). Priming and human memory systems. Science, 247, 301–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, M. C., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. (1987). Word recognition and comprehension skills in hyperlexic children. Brain and Language, 32, 76–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • West, R. F., Stanovich, K. E., Feeman, D., & Cunningham, A. (1983). The effect of sentence context on word recognition in second- and sixth-grade children. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 6–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sung Hee Lee.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lee, S.H., Hwang, M. Word and nonword processing without meaning support in Korean-speaking children with and without hyperlexia. Read Writ 28, 217–238 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-014-9522-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-014-9522-3

Keywords

Navigation