Skip to main content
Log in

Reading fluency skill and the prosodic marking of linguistic focus

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The purposes of the study were to determine whether third grade children mark linguistic focus features in their reading prosody and whether strong marking of these linguistic focus features might comprise an aspect of expressive reading typical of skilled, fluent reading. Children read a passage targeting information focusing aspects of prosody (direct quote, contrastive words, and exclamations). They also read a grade-level passage from the Qualitative Reading Inventory (Leslie & Caldwell, 2011) and completed the sight word subtest of Test of Word Reading Efficiency-2 (Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte, 2012). Children marked direct quote, contrastive words, and exclamations with a higher pitch than when in an unmarked context. They marked contrastive words and exclamations with greater intensity compared to an unmarked context. Further, reading fluency skill as determined by quick and accurate reading was connected to oral reading prosody that was generally more expressive for both sentence and linguistic focus prosodic features.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anolli, L., Ciceri, R., & Infantino, M. G. (2000). Irony as a game of implicitness: Acoustic profiles of ironic communication. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(3), 275–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, J., & Clifton, C. (2005). The prosodic property of lexical stress affects eye movements during silent reading. Cognition, 96, 89–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ayers, G. (1994). Discourse functions of pitch range in spontaneous and read speech. OSU Working Papers in Linguistics, 44, 1–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Banse, R., & Scherer, K. R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocabulary emotion expression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 614–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benjamin, R. G., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (2010). Text complexity and oral reading prosody in young readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 45(4), 388–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benjamin, R.G., Schwanenflugel, P. J., Meisinger, E. B., Groff, C., Kuhn, M. R., & Steiner, L. (2013). A Spectrographically Grounded Scale for Evaluating Reading Expressiveness. Reading Research Quarterly, 48(2), 105–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binder, K. S., Tighe, E., Jiang, Y., Kaftanski, K., Qi, C., & Ardoin, S. (2013). Reading expressively and understanding thoroughly: An examination of prosody in adults with low literacy. Reading and Writing, 26(5), 665–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blaauw, E. (1994). The contribution of prosodic boundary markers to the perceptual difference between read and spontaneous speech. Speech Communication, 14(4), 359–375. doi:10.1016/0167-6393(94)90028-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bodenbender, C. (1999). The punctuation and intonation of parentheticals. Unpublished masters thesis. Canada: University of Victoria.

  • Bolanos, D., Cole, R. A., Ward, W. H., Tindal, G. A., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (2013). Automatic assessment of expressive oral reading. Speech Communication, 55(2), 221–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, G. (1983). Prosodic structure and the given/new distinction. In Prosody, models and measurements (pp. 67–77). New York: Springer.

  • Bryant, G. A. (2010). Prosodic contrasts in ironic speech. Discourse Processes, 47(7), 545–566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, K., Dickey, M. W., Frazier, L., & Clifton, C. (2009). Information structure expectations in sentence comprehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 114–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowie, R., Douglas-Cowie, E., & Wichmann, A. (2002). Prosodic characteristics of skilled reading: Fluency and expressiveness in 8–10-year-old readers. Language and Speech, 45(1), 47–82. doi:10.1177/00238309020450010301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A., & Swinney, D. A. (1987). Prosody and the development of comprehension. Journal of Child Language, 14, 145–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehe, N. (2009). Clausal parentheticals, intonational phrasing, and prosodic theory. Journal of Linguistics, 45, 569–615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Den Ouden, H., Noordman, L., & Terken, J. (2009). Prosodic realizations of global and local structure on rhetorical relations in read aloud news reports. Speech Communication, 51, 116–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Esser, J., & Polomski, A. (1988). Comparing reading and speaking intonation. Amsterdam: Rodopi.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraundorf, S. H., Watson, D. G., & Benjamin, A. S. (2010). Recognition memory reveals just how CONTRASTIVE contrastive accenting really is. Journal of Memory and Language, 63(3), 367–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fujiki, M., Spackman, M. P., Brinton, B., & Illig, T. (2008). Ability of children with language impairment to understand emotion conveyed by prosody in a narrative passage. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 43(3), 330–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grosz, B., & Hirschberg, J. (1992, October). Some intonational characteristics of discourse structure. In Proceedings of ICSLP-92, Banff (pp. 429–432).

  • Guitierrez-Palma, N., & Palma-Reyes, A. (2005). On the use of lexical stress in reading Spanish. Reading & Writing, 21, 645–660.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirschberg, J. (2002). The pragmatics of intonational meaning. Conference presentation at Speech Prosody 2002. Aix-en-Provence, France.

  • Howell, P., & Kadi-Hanifi, K. (1991). Comparison of prosodic properties between read and spontaneous speech material. Speech Communication, 10(2), 163–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: Sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychological Methods, 3, 424–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu & Bentler. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Coventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, R., Pullen, P., Lane, H., & Torgesen, J. (2009). The complex nature of reading fluency: A multidimensional view. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 25(1), 4–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jansen, W., Gregory, M. L., & Brenier, J. M. (2001). Prosodic correlates of directly reported speech: Evidence from conversational speech. In Proceedings of the ISCA workshop on prosody in speech recognition and understanding (pp. 77–80). Redbank, NJ.

  • Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (2006). LISREL 8.80 for Windows [Computer Software]. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International, Inc.

  • Juslin, P. N., & Lauka, P. (2003). Communication of emotions in vocal expression and music performance. Psychological Bulletin, 129(5), 770–814.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katz, J., & Selkirk, E. O. (2011). Contrastive focus vs. discourse-new: Evidence from phonetic prominence in English. Language, 87(4), 771–816.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klauda, S. L., & Guthrie, J. T. (2008). Relationships of three components of reading fluency to reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(2), 310–321. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.100.2.310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kline, R. B. (2004). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (Ed. 2). NY: Guilford Press.

  • Kochanski, G., Grabe, E., Coleman, J., & Roser, B. (2005). Loudness predicts prominence: Fundamental frequency lends little. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 118, 1038–1051.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kral, P., Kleckova, J., & Cerisara, C. (2007). Sentence modality recognition in French based on prosody. World Academy of Science, Engineering, and Technology, 8, 677–680.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, M. R., Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Meisinger, E. B. (2010). Aligning theory and assessment of reading fluency: Automaticity, prosody, and definitions of fluency. Invited review article. Reading Research Quarterly, 45, 232–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, M. R., Schwanenflugel, P.J., Morris, R.D., Morrow, L. M., Bradley, B. A., Meisinger, E., Woo, D., & Stahl, S. A. (2006). Teaching children to become fluent and automatic readers. Journal of Literacy Research, 38, 357–387.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kutik, E., Cooper, W. E., & Boyce, S. (1983). Declination of fundamental frequency in speakers’ production of parenthetical and main clauses. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 73, 1731–1738.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, L., & Caldwell, J. (2011). Qualitative reading inventory-5 (QRI-5). Boston: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (2006). Prosody of syntactically complex sentences in the oral reading of young children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 839–853.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (2008). A longitudinal study of the development of reading prosody as a dimension of oral reading fluency in early elementary school children. Reading Research Quarterly, 43, 336–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, W. D., Rupley, W. H., & Rasinski, T. (2009). Fluency in learning to read for meaning: Going beyond repeated readings. Literacy Research and Instruction, 48(1), 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noordman, L., Dassen, I., Swets, M., & Terken, J. (1999). Prosodic markers of text structure. In K. van Hock, A. Kibrick, & L. Noordman (Eds.), Discourse studies in cognitive linguistics (pp. 133–145). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Patel, R., & Brayton, J. T. (2009). Identifying prosodic contrasts in utterances produced by 4-, 7-, and 11-year-old children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 790–801.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinnell, G. S., Pikulski, J. J., Wixson, K. K., Campbell, J. R., Gough, P. B., & Beatty, A. S. (1995). Listening to children read aloud: Data from NAEP’s Integrated reading performance record (IRPR) at grade 4 (NCES 95–726). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics, U.S. Department of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rasinski, T. (2004). Assessing reading fluency. Pacific Institute for Research and Evaluation.

  • Rasinski, T., Rikli, A., & Johnston, S. (2009). Reading fluency: More than automaticity? More than a concern for the primary grades? Literacy Research and Instruction, 48(4), 350–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwanenflugel, P. J., Hamilton, A. M., Kuhn, M. R., Wisenbaker, J. & Stahl, S. A. (2004). Becoming a fluent reader: Reading skill and prosodic features in the oral reading of young readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 119–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwanenflugel, P. J., Meisinger, E., Wisenbaker, J. M., Kuhn, M. R., Strauss, G. P., & Morris, R. D. (2006). Becoming a fluent and automatic reader in the early elementary school years. Reading Research Quarterly, 41, 496–522.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sluijter, A., & Terken, J. (1993). Beyond sentence prosody: Paragraph intonation in Dutch. Phonetica, 50, 180–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. L. (2004). Topic transitions and durational prosody in reading aloud: Production and modeling. Speech Communication, 42, 247–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sorianello, P. (2012, May). A prosodic account of Italian exclamative sentences: A gating test. Poster session at speech prosody, 6th international conference, Shanghai, China.

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (2012). Test of word reading efficiency -2 [Assessment kit]. Austin: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valencia, S., Smith, A., Reece, A., Li, M., Wixson, K., & Newman, H. (2010). Oral reading fluency assessment: Issues of construct, criterion, and consequential validity. Reading Research Quarterly, 45(3), 270–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, R.-M. (2008). The shape of direct quotation. Reading Teacher, 61(7), 558–564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, B., & Peppe, S. (2003). Intonation abilities of children with speech and language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 46(1), 5–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wennerstrom, A. (2001). The music of everyday speech: Prosody and discourse analysis. London: Oxford University Press.

  • Wilson, D., & Wharton, T. (2006). Relevance and prosody. Journal of Pragmatics, 38, 1559–1579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yagoda, B. (2012, August 6). The point of exclamation. The New York Times. http://opinionator.blogs.nytimes.com/2012/08/06/the-point-of-exclamation/.

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank the children of Barrow County, GA, and Hazlet, NJ for their participation in this study. We thank Lilly Steiner, Carolyn Groff, Stephanie Lai, and Adam Reed for collecting some of the data reported here. Some of the information reported here was presented previously in July, 2012 at the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading meeting in Montreal, Quebec CANADA. Correspondence regarding this manuscript should be sent to Dr. Paula J. Schwanenflugel, Department of Educational Psychology, 325 Aderhold Hall, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602; pschwan@uga.edu.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paula J. Schwanenflugel.

Appendices

Appendix 1

Frog and Toad have a New Friend

Frog and Toad were good, happy funny friends. They did all kinds of activities in the forest. They would like to walk slowly together at first and then have an increase in their speed. Then, Frog would run with Toad from the woods. They liked to recall all the lovely times they had playing in the forest.

One day, Toad found two paths ahead (PI1-U) and he was not clear where they went. (DS-1) They considered both. Pointing down hill, Frog said, “Let’s try (CS1-M) that.” But Toad wasn’t sure whether to take (CS2-U) this one. It was on this path that Frog (PI1-M) (who can get himself into trouble sometimes) set out anyway.

Toad asked him to (QS1-U) come back. But Frog was gone. Toad repeated, “Please (QS1-M) come back.”

Frog came upon a small green house. It looked empty. There were neat, pretty, colorful flowers growing along a fence. Maybe an artist lived there.

Soon, Toad gave up and followed him. Toad said, “(QS2-M) It doesn’t seem that there is anyone inside. (Y-NQ1 Is anyone inside?”

Frog said (QS2-U) it doesn’t seem that there is. (QS2) Frog had an (EP1-U) idea. Frog said, “We should go and look.” What an (EP1-M) idea!

“We should go and look? I don’t know,” replied Toad. It looked like somebody might live there. “Let’s wait to see if anyone comes home.”

They waited and waited. They watched the animals. Bunnies were jumping. Cats were sleeping in the grass. Mice were making nests under old logs. A blackbird flew by. Frog said, “I almost missed (CS1-U) that.” All the animals were active.

Then, a tall man walked over to them (PI2-M) (and he took them by surprise).

“Hi, I am Toad and this is Frog. What’s your name?” asked Toad. He smiled a (EP-2 U) real big smile.

“I am Big John,” said the man. He saw them gazing at his garden. (Y-NQ-2) “Would you like to see my garden?”

Frog said, “We would like that very much.”

Big John decided which path to take. Big John decided against the path by the garden fence. He didn’t walk on this path. It was wet. In fact, a black bird was taking a bath by the garden fence. So, he chose the path that went through a small greenhouse.

There were many neat, pretty colorful flowers in the garden. It was very artistic. Big John saw Toad looking at his flowers. “Would you like one?” he asked. Big John held up two, a blue one and a yellow one. He held out one flower that was in his garden and said, “Here, take (CS2-M) this one.” He gave Toad the blue one. So, it was in his garden that they all became friends.

“I’m so glad to have new friends,” said Big John. Now, they all had an increase in good friends. Their recall of that day always brought a (EP2-M) real big smile! To this day, they are still good, happy, funny friends. Frog and Toad (PI1-U) who can now find their way to Big John’s house will always visit their friend from the woods

Appendix 2

See Table 4.

Table 4 Correlations among prosody variables

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Schwanenflugel, P.J., Westmoreland, M.R. & Benjamin, R.G. Reading fluency skill and the prosodic marking of linguistic focus. Read Writ 28, 9–30 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-013-9456-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-013-9456-1

Keywords

Navigation