Skip to main content
Log in

Do dyslexics misread a ROWS for a ROSE?

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Insufficient knowledge of the subtle relations between words’ spellings and their phonology is widely held to be the primary limitation in developmental dyslexia. In the present study the influence of phonology on a semantic-based reading task was compared for groups of readers with and without dyslexia. As many studies have shown, skilled readers make phonology-based false-positive errors to homophones and pseudohomophones in the semantic categorization task. The basic finding was extended to children, teens, and adults with dyslexia from familial and clinically-referred samples. Dyslexics showed the same overall pattern of phonology errors and the results were consistent across dyslexia samples, across age groups, and across experimental conditions using word and nonword homophone foils. The dyslexic groups differed from chronological-age matched controls by having elevated false-positive homophone error rates overall, and weaker effects of baseword frequency. Children with dyslexia also made more false-positive errors to spelling control foils. These findings suggest that individuals with dyslexia make use of phonology when making semantic decisions both to word homophone and non-word pseudohomophone foils and that dyslexics lack adequate knowledge of actual word spellings, compared to chronological-age and reading-level matched control participants.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barker, T. A., Torgesen, J. K., & Wagner, R. K. (1992). The role of orthographic processing skills on five different reading tasks. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 334–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (1994). Multiple orthographic and phonological codes in literacy acquisition: An evolving research program. In V. W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge I: Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 359–380). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V., Abbott, R., Thomson, J., Wagner, R., Swanson, H. L., Wijsman, E., et al. (2006). Modeling developmental phonological core deficits within a working-memory architecture in children and adults with developmental dyslexia. Scientific Studies in Reading, 10, 165–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Betjemann, R. S., & Keenan, J. M. (2008). Phonological and semantic priming in children with reading disability. Child Development, 79(4), 1086–1102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bosman, A. M. T., & Van Orden, G. (1997). Why spelling is more difficult than reading. In C. A. Perfetti, L. Rieben, & M. Fayol (Eds.), Learning to spell (pp. 173–194). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bosman, A. M. T., Vonk, W., & van Zwam, M. (2006). Spelling consistency affects reading in young Dutch readers with and without dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 56(2), 271–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. (1978). Difficulties in auditory organization as a possible cause of reading backwardness. Nature, 271, 746–747.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read: A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brkanac, Z., Chapman, N. H., Igo, R. P., Matsushita, M. M., Nielsen, K., Berninger, V. W., et al. (2008). Genome scan of a nonword repetition phenotype in families with dyslexia: Evidence for multiple loci. Behavioral Genetics, 38(5), 462–475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M. (1993). Component spelling skills of college students with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia. Learning Disability Quarterly, 16(3), 171–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cardon, L. R., DeFries, J. C., Fulker, D. W., Kimberling, W. J., Pennington, B. F., & Smith, S. D. (1994). Quantitative trait locus for reading disability on chromosome 6. Science, 265, 276–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 47, 149–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castles, A., Datta, H., Gayan, J., & Olson, R. K. (1999). Varieties of developmental reading disorder: Genetic and environmental influences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72, 73–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castles, A., Davis, C., Cavalot, P., & Forster, K. (2007). Tracking the acquisition of orthographic skills in developing readers: Masked priming effects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97(3), 165–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Connelly, V., Campbell, S., MacLean, M., & Barnes, J. (2006). Contribution of lower order skills to the written composition of college students with and without dyslexia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29(1), 175–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1990). Assessing print exposure and orthographic processing skill in children: A quick measure of reading experience. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(4), 733–740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curtin, S., Manis, F. R., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2001). Parallels between the reading and spelling deficits of two subgroups of developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 515–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, C. J., Perea, M., & Acha, J. (2009). Re(de)fining the orthographic neighborhood: The role of addition and deletion neighbors in lexical decision and reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(5), 1550–1570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., & Markwardt, F. C. (1970). Examiner’s manual: Peabody individual achievement test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. (1992). Reconceptualizing the development of sight word reading and its relationship to recoding. Reading Acquisition, 5, 107–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. (1995). Phases of development in learning to read words by sight. Journal of Research in Reading, 18, 116–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. (2005). Learning to read words: Theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9(2), 167–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C., & Saltmarsh, J. (1995). Beginning readers outperform older disabled readers in learning to read words by sight. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7(3), 295–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elbro, C., Borstrom, I., & Petersen, D. (1998). Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten: The importance of distinctiveness of phonological representations of lexical items. Reading Research Quarterly, 33, 36–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farrar, W. T., Van Orden, G., & Hamouz, V. (2001). When SOFA primes TOUCH: Interdependence of spelling, sound, and meaning in “semantically mediated” phonological priming. Memory & Cognition, 29(3), 530–539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finucci, J. M. (1986). Follow-up studies of developmental dyslexia and other learning disabilities. In S. D. Smith (Ed.), Genetics and learning disabilities (pp. 97–121). Philadelphia, PA: Taylor and Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gayán, J., & Olson, R. K. (2003). Genetic and environmental influences on individual differences in printed word recognition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84(2), 97–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., & Willows, D. (1994). Orthographic knowledge is orthographic knowledge is orthographic knowledge. In V. W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge I: Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 359–380). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, S., & Kennemer, K. (2005). Learning disabilities. In S. Goldstein & C. R. Reynolods (Eds.), Handbook of neurodevelopmental and genetic disorders in adults (pp. 91–114). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottlob, L. R., Goldinger, S. D., Stone, G. O., & Van Orden, G. (1999). Reading homographs: Orthographic, phonologic, and semantic dynamics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 561–574.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harm, M., & Seidenberg, M. (1999). Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: Insights from connectionist models. Psychological Review, 106, 491–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hollis, G. (2010). Observed interdependence of cognition and action: The hand says ‘no’ to ROWS. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH.

  • Keenan, J. M., & Betjemann, R. S. (2008). Comprehension of single words: The role of semantics in word identification and reading disability. In E. Grigorenko & A. Naples (Eds.), Single word reading (pp. 191–210). New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I., Olofsson, A., & Wall, S. (1980). Reading and spelling skills in the first school years predicted from phonemic awareness skills in kindergarten. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 21, 159–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F., Seidenberg, M. S., Doi, L., McBride-Chang, C., & Petersen, A. (1996). On the basis of two subtypes of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 58, 157–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marinelli, C. V., Angelelli, P., Notarnicola, A., & Luzzatti, C. (2009). Do Italian dyslexic children use the lexical reading route efficiently? An orthographic judgment task. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22, 333–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miles, T. R. (1983). Dyslexia: The pattern of difficulties. London: Granada.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (1998). Individual differences in contextual facilitation: Evidence from dyslexics and poor reading compensation. Child Development, 69, 996–1009.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R. K., Kliegl, R., Davidson, B. J., & Foltz, G. (1985). Individual and developmental differences in reading disability. In G. E. MacKinnon & T. G. Waller (Eds.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice (Vol. 4, pp. 1–64). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R. K., Wise, B., Conners, F., Rack, J., & Fulker, D. (1989). Specific deficits in component reading and language skills: Genetic and environmental influences. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 339–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., Cardoso-Martins, C., Green, P. A., & Lefly, D. L. (2001). Comparing the phonological and double deficit hypothesis for developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 707–755.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., Lefly, D. L., Van Orden, G., Bookman, M. O., & Smith, S. D. (1987). Is phonology bypassed in normal or dyslexic development? Annals of Dyslexia, 37, 62–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., McCabe, L. L., Smith, S. D., Lefly, D. L., Bookman, M. O., Kimberling, W. J., et al. (1986). Spelling errors in adults with a form familial dyslexia. Child Development, 57, 1001–1013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., Van Orden, G., Smith, S. D., Green, P. A., & Haith, M. M. (1990). Phonological processing skills and deficits in adult dyslexics. Child Development, 61, 1753–1778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), reading acquisition (pp. 145–174). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. In L. Verhoeven, C. Elbro, & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (pp. 189–213). Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plaut, D. (1997). Structure and function in the lexical system: Insights from distributed models of naming and lexical decision. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12, 767–808.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rack, J. P., Snowling, M. J., & Olson, R. K. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: A review. Reading Research Quarterly, 27(1), 28–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raskind, W. H., Igo, R. P., Chapman, N. H., Berninger, V. W., Thomson, J. B., Matsushita, M., et al. (2005). A genome scan in multigenerational families with dyslexia: Identification of a novel locus on chromosome 2q that contributes to phonological decoding efficiency. Molecular Psychiatry, 10, 699–711.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1988). Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. London: H. K. Lewis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reitsma, P. (1983). Printed word learning in beginning readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 36(2), 321–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. S. (1984). Continuity between childhood dyslexia and adult reading. British Journal of Psychology, 75, 329–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. S. (1990). Very early language deficits in dyslexic children. Child Development, 61, 1728–1743.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schonhaut, S., & Satz, P. (1983). Prognosis for children with learning disabilities: A review of follow-up studies. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Developmental neuropsychiatry (pp. 542–563). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55(2), 151–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (2008). Orthographic learning, phonological recoding, and self-teaching. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 36, pp. 31–82). Amsterdam: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., & Shalev, C. (2004). Self-teaching in normal and disabled readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17, 769–800.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. D., Kimberling, W. J., Pennington, B. F., & Lubs, H. A. (1983). Specific reading disability: Identification of an inherited form from linkage analysis. Science, 219, 1345–1347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. D., Pennington, B. F., Kimberling, W. J., & Ing, P. S. (1990). Familial dyslexia: Use of genetic linkage data to define subtypes. Journal of the American Academy of Teen and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 204–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J. (1981). Phonemic deficits in developmental dyslexia. Psychological Research, 43, 219–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1989). Exposure to print and orthographic processing. Reading Research Quarterly, 24(4), 402–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., West, R. F., & Freeman, D. J. (1981). A longitudinal study of sentence context effects in second-grade children: Tests of an interactive-compensatory model. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 32, 185–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Orden, G. (1987). A ROWS is a ROSE: Spelling, sound, and reading. Memory & Cognition, 15(3), 181–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Orden, G., & Goldinger, S. D. (1994). Interdependence of form and function in cognitive systems explains perception of printed words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 1269–1291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Orden, G., Johnston, J. C., & Hale, B. L. (1988). Word identification in reading proceeds from spelling to sound to meaning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14(3), 371–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Orden, G., & Kloos, H. (2005). The question of phonology and reading. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 61–78). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Van Orden, G., Pennington, B. F., & Stone, G. O. (1990). Word identification in reading and the promise of subsymbolic psycholinguistics. Psychological Review, 97(4), 488–522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler intelligence scale for children—Revised. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeno, S. M., Ivens, S. H., Millard, R. T., & Duvvuri, R. (1995). The educator’s word frequency guide. Brewster, NY: Touchstone Applied Science Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Data collection was supported by an award of research funds from The Developmental Psychobiology Endowment Fund, University of Colorado School of Medicine, to Guy Van Orden; preparation of this article was supported by an NSF grant (BCS #0843133), also to Guy Van Orden.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Beth A. O’Brien.

Appendices

Appendix 1

 

Category

Homophone foil

Spelling control

Words

 A part of a building

Seller

Teller

 A part of a building

Stare

Start

 A non-alcoholic beverage

Tee

Ten

 A carpenter’s tool

Plain

Place

 A metal

Steal

Steep

 A part of a lion’s body

Tale

Talk

 A four-footed animal

Dear

Dyer

 A body of water

See

Set

 A vegetable

Beats

Belts

 A flower

Rows

Robs

Nonwords

 A vegetable

Karret

Sarret

 A four-footed animal

Sheap

Shelp

 A tree

Oke

Onk

 A part of the human body

Brane

Brafe

 An article of clothing

Shurt

Shart

 A place of confinement

Jale

Jalk

 A vehicle

Jeap

Jelp

 Things in a woman’s purse

Kee

Ket

 A weather phenomenon

Sleat

Slert

 A kitchen utensil

Bole

Bolf

Appendix 2

 

Category

Homophone foil

Spelling control

Low frequency

 A place to shop

Maul

Mull

 A four-footed animal

Dear

Dyer

 Part of a ship

Sale

Soil

 A part of a bike

Break

Brave

 An insect

Flee

Fled

 A vegetable

Beats

Belts

 A part of a horse’s body

Main

Many

 A wild animal

Bore

Born

 A part of a dress

Seem

Slam

 A small stream

Creak

Cheek

High frequency

 Part of a house

Haul

Hail

 Part of the human body

Feat

Felt

 A color

Blew

Bled

 A carpenter’s tool

Plain

Place

 A four-footed animal

Bare

Beer

 A metal

Steal

Steep

 A servant

Made

Mail

 A part of the human body

Hare

Harp

 Type of food

Meet

Melt

 A feature of an ocean shore

Beech

Belch

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

O’Brien, B.A., Van Orden, G.C. & Pennington, B.F. Do dyslexics misread a ROWS for a ROSE?. Read Writ 26, 381–402 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9373-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9373-8

Keywords

Navigation