Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Does whole-word multimedia software support literacy acquisition?

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined the extent to which multimedia features of typical literacy learning software provide added benefits for developing literacy skills compared with typical whole-class teaching methods. The effectiveness of the multimedia software Oxford Reading Tree (ORT) for Clicker in supporting early literacy acquisition was investigated through a classroom-based intervention study by comparing ORT for Clicker to traditional ORT Big Books. Sixty-one typically developing readers, aged 5–6 years, from four primary classes were each given the two interventions. Each intervention was delivered over five one-hour sessions over the course of 1 week. Performance on tasks of written word recognition, written word naming, and phonological awareness, was measured before and after each intervention. Significantly greater gains in word recognition, word naming, rhyme awareness, segmentation skill, and grapheme awareness were found after the ORT for Clicker compared to the traditional ORT Big Book intervention. This study shows that whole-word multimedia software can be more effective than traditional printed texts from the same reading scheme at supporting the development of early literacy skills.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Kolmogorov–Smirnov test was significant (p < .05, at least) for some of the baseline measures (i.e., LDT nonwords, LDT all letter strings, rhyme awareness, and grapheme awareness) indicating that these distributions deviated significantly from normality. However, non-parametric analyses, using Friedman’s test followed by Wilcoxon Signed Rank tests, gave the same pattern of results as the parametric analyses reported in text.

References

  • Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking & learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, S. (2006). From inkmarks to ideas: Current issues in lexical processing. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P. E., Maclean, M., & Bradley, L. (1990). Rhyme, language and children’s reading. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 237–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (2004). Is there a causal link from phonological awareness to success in learning to read? Cognition, 91, 77–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castles, A., & Nation, K. (2006). How does orthographic learning happen? In S. Andrews (Ed.), From inkmarks to ideas: Current issues in lexical processing (pp. 151–179). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chera, P., & Wood, C. (2003). Animated multimedia ‘talking books’ can promote phonological awareness in children beginning to read. Learning and Instruction, 13, 33–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cox, M. J., & Abbott, C. (2004). ICT and attainment: A review of the research literature. Coventry and London: British Educational Communications and Technology Agency/Department for Education and Skills.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crick Software. (2006). Oxford reading tree for clicker. Northampton, UK: Crick Software Ltd.

  • DeJean, J., Miller, L., & Olson, J. (1999). CD-ROM talking books: What do they promise? Education and Information Technologies, 2, 121–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Jong, M. T., & Bus, A. G. (2002). Quality of book reading matters for emergent readers: An experiment with the same book in a regular or electronic format. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 145–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Jong, M. T., & Bus, A. G. (2003). How well suited are electronic books to supporting literacy? Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 3, 147–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dynarski, M., Agodini, R., Heaviside, S., Novak, T., Carey, N., Campuzano, L., Means, B., Murphy, R., Penuel, W., Javitz, H., Emery, D., & Sussex, W. (2007). Effectiveness of reading and mathematics software products: Findings from the first student cohort. Retrieved August 13, 2008, from http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/pubs/20074005/.

  • Ehri, L. C. (1989). The development of spelling knowledge and its role in reading acquisition and reading disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 356–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzer, K. M., Freidhoff, J. R., Fritzen, A., Heintz, A., Koehler, J., Mishra, P., et al. (2007). Guest editorial: More questions than answers: Responding to the reading and mathematics software effectiveness study. Contemporary Issues in Technology and Teacher Education, 7(2), 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, A. E. (1991). How early phonological development might set the stage for phoneme awareness. In S. A. Brady & D. P. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman (pp. 97–117). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartley, J. (2007). Teaching, learning, and new technology: A review for teachers. British Journal of Educational Technology, 36, 42–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, R., & Brychta, A. (2003a). Strawberry jam. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, R., & Brychta, A. (2003b). Kipper the clown. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, R., & Page, T. (2003). Teacher’s handbook; stages 1–9. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Korat, O., & Shamir, A. (2004). Do Hebrew electronic books differ from Dutch electronic books? A replication of a Dutch content analysis. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 20, 257–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Korat, O., & Shamir, A. (2007). Electronic books versus adult readers: Effects on children’s emergent literacy as a function of social class. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 23, 248–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labbo, L. D., & Kuhn, M. R. (2000). Weaving chains of affect and cognition: A young child’s understanding of CD-ROM talking books. Journal of Literacy Research, 32, 187–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, C. (2000). Exploring the effects of talking book software in UK primary classrooms. Journal of Research in Reading, 23, 149–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Littleton, K., Wood, C., & Chera, P. (2006). Interactions with talking books: Phonological awareness affects boys’ use of talking books. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 22, 382–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Longman. (1999). Bangers and mash. London, UK: Longman Group Limited.

  • Mann, V. A., & Wimmer, H. (2002). Phoneme awareness and pathways to literacy: A comparison of German and American children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 653–682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthew, K. I. (1996). The impact of CD-ROM Story books on children’s reading comprehension and reading attitude. Journal of Educational Multimedia and Hypermedia, 5, 379–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKenna, M. C. (1998). Electronic texts and the transformation of beginning reading. In D. Reinking, M. C. McKenna, L. D. Labbo, & R. D. Kieffer (Eds.), Handbook of literacy and technology: Transformation in a post-typographic world (pp. 45–59). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKenna, M. C., Reinking, D., & Bradley, B. A. (2003). The effects of electronic trade books on the decoding growth of beginning readers. In R. M. Joshi, C. K. Leong, & B. L. J. Kaczmarek (Eds.), Literacy acquisition: The role of phonology, morphology, and orthography (pp. 193–202). Amsterdam: IOS Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, L., Blackstock, J., & Miller, R. (1994). An exploratory study into the use of CD-ROM storybooks. Computers & Education, 22, 187–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morais, J. (1991). Phonological awareness: A bridge between language and literacy. In D. Sawyer & B. Fox (Eds.), Phonological awareness in reading: The evolution of current perspectives (pp. 31–71). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Office for Standards in Education (Ofsted). (2006). Inspection Report. Retrieved June 6, 2006, from http://www.ofsted.gov.uk

  • Office for Standards in Education (Ofsted). (2007a). Inspection Report. Retrieved February 20, 2007, from http://www.ofsted.gov.uk

  • Office for Standards in Education (Ofsted). (2007b). Inspection Report. Retrieved October 10, 2007, from http://www.ofsted.gov.uk

  • Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (1989). The psychology of reading. New York: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, C., & Salter, W. (1997). The phonological awareness test. East Moline, IL: LinguiSystems.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D., & Fowler, A. E. (2004). Questions people ask about the role of phonological processes in learning to read. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17, 483–515.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M., & Hulme, C. (2005). The science of reading: A handbook. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1999). Who is rational? Studies of individual differences in reasoning. Mahway, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teale, W. (1981). Parents reading to their children: What we know and what we need to know. Language Arts, 58, 902–912.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgerson, C. J., & Zhu, D. (2004). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effectiveness of ICT on literacy learning in English. In R. Andrews (Ed.), The impact of ICT on literacy education (pp. 5–16). London: RoutledgeFalmer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, J. D. M. (2000). Computer support for reading development. Journal of Research in Reading, 23, 136–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, G., & Underwood, J. D. M. (1998). Children’s interactions and learning outcomes with interactive talking books. Computers & Education, 30, 95–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walley, A. C., Metsala, J. L., & Garlock, V. M. (2003). Spoken vocabulary growth: Its role in the development of phoneme awareness and early reading ability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 5–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Ann Crick of Crick Software for her ideas and suggestions. We also thank the staff and students of St. Teresa’s Catholic Primary School, Heymann Primary School and Radford Primary School for participating in the study and Maria Ktori, Susannah Kempson and Katherine Siu for their help with data collection. This research was supported by an ESRC Case PhD studentship awarded to Dr. Nicola Pitchford and Professor Claire O’Malley, co-sponsored by Crick Software (grant number PTA-033-2004-00064).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nicola J. Pitchford.

Appendices

Appendix A

Stimuli used in the lexical decision and oral reading tasks

Reception words

ORT intervention words

Year-1 words

Year-2 words

Words

Matched nonwords

Words

Matched nonwords

Words

Matched nonwords

Words

Matched nonwords

and

they

big

see

said

ind

thim

cag

teeg

faip

about

an

had

help

home

make

over

put

ran

time

too

took

want

wanted

your

after

be

did

good

laugh

made

man

pull

pulled

what

abond

en

dal

welk

wame

mage

ovel

pid

rop

bime

noo

hoos

nand

wented

yurt

dawoe

ob

tul

harc

healt

yade

mun

grel

heapel

whut

bed

his

more

then

were

once

night

some

came

down

det

hin

moso

wesb

lere

kon

yatoe

shom

hame

drot

yellow

eight

where

wednesday

these

yelter

teigh

whust

wednesdau

thamp

Appendix B

Literacy hour intervention plan

Session

Duration (min)

Group

Focus

Activity

Shared work

15

Whole-class

Focus is on exploration and reading aloud of the story

Big Book: involved reading the story in front of the class and encouraging the children to look at the words and pictures and to read it aloud with the teacher

ORT for Clicker: involved reading the story on the interactive white board whilst encouraging the children to look at the pictures and highlighted words and to read the words aloud whilst spoken by the narrator

Word/sentence level work

15

Whole-class

Focus is on a particular part of reading (e.g., grammar & punctuation, vocabulary, or spelling)

Involved activities such as spotting mistakes in sentences, making a word bank, creating new sentences with target words, and completing sentences on the interactive white board activities (ORT for Clicker) or non-interactive white board activities (Big Book)

Focus

20

Individual

Focus is on activities at the word-level, sentence level and comprehension

Big Book: children used traditional books, pencil and paper.

ORT for Clicker: children worked through a CD-ROM activity on individual PC laptops

Involved identifying spelling patterns, reading and matching words, becoming familiar with word order, writing simple sentences and answering questions based on the story content, and recognizing and reading high frequency words in the book (Big Book) or through listening and finding high frequency words on the screen (ORT for Clicker)

Plenary session

10

Whole-class

Focus on what was taught during the literacy activity

Both interventions: evaluation of what was taught in the literacy hour

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Karemaker, A.M., Pitchford, N.J. & O’Malley, C. Does whole-word multimedia software support literacy acquisition?. Read Writ 23, 31–51 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-008-9148-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-008-9148-4

Keywords

Navigation