Skip to main content
Log in

Phonological and lexical reading in Italian children with dyslexia

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this study we explore the development of phonological and lexical reading in dyslexic children. We tested a group of 14 Italian children who have been diagnosed with dyslexia and whose reading age is end of grade 1. We compared this group with a group of 70 typically developing children who have been tested for reading at the end of grade 1. For each dyslexic child we also selected a participant who was attending the same grade, was close in age, and showed typical reading development when tested with a narrative passage reading task (Cornoldi, Colpo, & Gruppo MT, 1981) for correctness and reading speed. Children in this group are “same grade controls.” We used a reading task consisting of 40 three syllables words. A qualitative and quantitative method of coding children’s naming allowed us to distinguish several components of their reading performance: the grapheme and word recognition, the size of orthographic units involved in the aloud orthography–phonology conversion, the reading process used to recognize words. The comparison of the dyslexic group with the reading age and the same grade control groups reveals different trends of delayed reading processes. Considering dyslexic children’s chronological age, lexical reading is greatly delayed. Considering dyslexic children’s reading age, the type of reading process that is more deeply delayed is phonological reading. The rate of fragmented phonological reading (i.e., a type of syllabized phonological reading) is much higher in dyslexic children compared to the reading age group, suggesting that some factors undermine the possibility of internalizing the orthography–phonology conversion and the blending processes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barca, L., Burani, C., & Arduino, L. S. (2002). Word naming times and psycholinguistic norms for Italian nouns. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 34, 424–434.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barca, L., Burani, C., Di Filippo, G., & Zoccolotti, P. (2006). Italian developmental dyslexic and proficient readers: Where are the differences? Brain & Language, 98, 347–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burani, C., & Arduino, L. S. (2004). Stress regularity or consistency? Reading aloud Italian polysyllables with different stress patterns. Brain and Language, 90, 318–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burani, C., Marcolini, S., & Stella, G. (2002). How early does morpho-lexical reading develop in readers of a shallow orthography? Brain & Language, 81, 568–586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 47, 149–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chilosi, A. M., Lami, L., Pizzoli, C., Pignatti, B., D’Alessandro, D., Gruppioni, B., et al. (2003). Profili neuropsicologici nella dislessia evolutiva (Neuropsychological profiles in developmental dyslexia). Psicologia Clinica dello Sviluppo, 2, 269–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colombo, L. (1992). Lexical stress effect and its interaction with frequency in word pronunciation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 978–1003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colombo, L., & Tabossi, P. (1992). Strategies and stress assignment: evidence from a shallow orthography. In R. Frost & L. Katz (Eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology and meaning (pp. 319–339). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cornoldi, C., Colpo, M., & Gruppo, M. T. (1981). La verifica dell’apprendimento della lettura (Assessment of reading acquisition). Firenze, Italy: Edizioni O.S.

  • Cunningham, A. E., Perry, K. E., Stanovich, K. E., & Share, D. L. (2002). Orthographic learning during reading: examining the role of self-teaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 185–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Luca, M., Borrelli, M., Judica, A., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2002). Reading words and pseudowords: An eye movement study of developmental dyslexia. Brain and Language, 80, 617–626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Luca, M., Di Pace, E., Judica, A., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (1999). Eye movement patterns in linguistic and non-linguistic tasks in developmental surface dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 37, 1407–1420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Facoetti, A., Zorzi, M., Cestnick, L., Lorusso, M. L., Molteni, M., Paganoni, P., et al. (2006). The relationship between visuo-spatial attention and nonword reading in developmental dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23(6), 841–855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K. Patterson, J. Marshall & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface dyslexia (pp. 301–330). London: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonzalez, J. E. J., & Hernàndez Valle, I. (2000). Word identification and reading disorders in the Spanish language. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 44–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., Gombert, J. E., & Fraca de Barrera, L. (1998). Children’s orthographic representations and linguistic transparency: Nonsense word reading in English, French and Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 19–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutzler, F., & Wimmer, H. (2004). Eye movements of dyslexic children when reading in a regular orthography. Brain and Language, 89, 235–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Job, R., Peressotti, F., & Mulatti, C. (2005). The acquisition of literacy in Italian. In R. Malatesha Joshi & P. G. Aaron (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 105–119). Lawrence Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kyte, C. S., & Johnson, C. J. (2006). The role of phonological recoding in orthographic learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93(2), 166–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landerl, K., Wimmer, H., & Frith, U. (1997). The impact of orthographic consistency on dyslexia: A German-English comparison. Cognition, 63, 315–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., Doi, L. M., McBride-Chang, C., & Peterson, A. (1996). On the bases of two subtypes of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 58, 157–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marconi, L., Ott, M., Pesenti, E., Ratti, D., & Tavella, M. (1993). Lessico Elementare. Dati statistici sull’italiano letto e scritto dai bambini delle elementari (Elementary lexicon. Statistical data on the Italian that elementary school children read and write). Bologna, Italy: Zanichelli.

  • McDougall, P., Borowsky, R., MacKinnon, G. E., & Hymel, S. (2005). Process dissociation of sight vocabulary and phonetic decoding in reading: A new perspective on surface and phonological dyslexias. Brain and Language, 92, 185–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Beyond phonological skills: broader language skills contribute to the development of reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 27, 342–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orsolini, M., Fanari, R., Famiglietti, L., & Maronato, C. (submitted). Assessing reading development in Italian children: from phonological to lexical reading.

  • Orsolini, M., Fanari, R., Tosi, V., De Nigris, B., & Carrieri, R. (2006). From phonological recoding to lexical reading: a longitudinal study on reading development in Italian. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21(5), 576–607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paulesu, E., Demonet, J., Fazio, F., McCrory, E., Chanoine, V., Brunswick, N., et al. (2001). Dyslexia: cultural diversity and biological unity. Science, 291, 2165–2167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Porpodas, C. D. (1999). Patterns of phonological and memory processing in beginning readers and spellers of Greek. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 406–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rack, J. P., Snowling, M. J., & Olson, R. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: A review. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 29–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, B. F., & Bakeman, R. (1998). ComKappa: A Windows 95 program for calculating kappa and related statistics. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 30, 731–732.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sartori, G., Job, R., & Tressoldi, P. E. (1995). Batteria per la valutazione della dislessia e della disortografia evolutiva (Battery for assessing developmental dyslexia and disorthography). Florence, Italy: Edizioni O.S.

    Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., & Shalev, C. (2004). Self-teaching in normal and disabled readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17(7–8), 769–800.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J. (2000). Dyslexia. Oxford, England: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J. (2001). Developmental dyslexia. Current Pediatrics, 11, 10–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spinelli, D., De Luca, M., Di Filippo, G., Mancini, M., Martelli, M., & Zoccolotti, P. (2005). Length effects in word naming in reading: Role of reading experience and reading deficit in Italian readers. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(2), 217–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sprenger-Charolles, L., Colé, P., Lacert, P., & Serniclaes, W. (2000). On subtypes of developmental dyslexia: Evidence from processing time and accuracy scores. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 87–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Siegel, L. S. (1997). A longitudinal study of the effects of syllabic structure on the development of reading and spelling skills in French. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 485–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tabossi, P., & Laghi, L. (1992). Semantic priming in the pronunciation of words in two writing systems: Italian and English. Memory & Cognition, 20(3), 303–313.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1974). WISC-R Manual for Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children-Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation (Italian version: Rubini, V., & Padovani, F. (1986) WISC-R Scala di intelligenza Wechsler per bambini - Riveduta. Firenze: O.S).

  • Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Ma-Wyatt, A., Ladner, D., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2003). Developmental dyslexia in different languages: Language specific or universal? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 169–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by a grant from MIUR to M. Orsolini. We thank Pierluigi Zoccolotti, Cristina Burani, Anna Judica for their helpful comments. The authors thank Sara Moretti and Roberta di Nepi for helping in transcribing and coding the reading responses of children with dyslexia. The authors are grateful to the teachers, staff and the children of the primary schools in Rome who participated at the research.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Margherita Orsolini.

Appendices

Appendix A

Our reading list consists of 40 three syllable words and includes 20 high frequency (HF) and 20 low frequency (LF) words. Both the high frequency and low frequency sublists include 10 atypical (AS) and 10 typical (TS) stress assignment words. The range of the words length in letters is 6–8. The four sublists (HF-AS; HF-TS; LF-AS; LF-TS) have been balanced in terms of age of acquisition, familiarity, imageability, concreteness (norms drawn from Barca, Burani, & Arduino, 2002) and orthographic neighbourhood, as shown below (mean and standard deviation).

 

Age of acquisition

Familiarity

Imageability

Concreteness

Orthographic neighbourhood

High freq-atypical stress

2.4 (.55)

6.6 (.20)

5.5 (.75)

5.5 (1.23)

.7 (.67)

High freq-typical stress

2.9 (.40)

6.7 (.15)

5.4 (.65)

5.3 (1.41)

.7 (.48)

Low freq-atypical stress

3.7 (.59)

6.2 (.35)

5.2 (.97)

5.7 (.85)

.5 (.71)

Low freq typical stress

3.8 (.68)

5.8 (.36)

5.4 (.97)

5.7 (.96)

.6 (.97)

The word set along with word frequency and bigram frequency is shown below

Typical stress

Word Freq.

Bigram freq.

Atypical stress

Word freq.

Bigram freq.

Canzone (song)

HF

178

10.88

Angolo (corner)

HF

173

10.73

Compagno (mate)

HF

748

10.77

Favola (tale)

HF

72

10.86

Estate (summer)

HF

377

11.33

Macchina (car)

HF

807

10.89

Farina (flour)

HF

88

11.12

Nuvola (cloud)

HF

222

10.14

Natura (nature)

HF

248

10.92

Pagina (page)

HF

139

10.96

Nipote (nephew)

HF

119

10.58

Pecora (sheep)

HF

80

11.22

Parete (wall)

HF

81

11.26

Polvere (dust)

HF

61

10.85

Patata (potato)

HF

66

11.32

Scatola (box)

HF

137

11.24

Stagione (season)

HF

202

11.15

Tavolo (table)

HF

129

10.92

Tappeto (carpet)

HF

71

10.72

Zucchero (sugar)

HF

71

10.02

Carciofo (artichoke)

LF

0

10.53

Brivido (shiver)

LF

17

10.34

Cometa (comet)

LF

9

11.22

Carcere (prison)

LF

9

11.18

Cratere (crater)

LF

7

11.28

Gomito (elbow)

LF

11

10.69

Galera (prison)

LF

15

11.10

Incubo (nightmare)

LF

25

10.17

Imbuto (funnel)

LF

0

10.37

Mandorla (almond)

LF

22

10.96

Menzogna (deceipt)

LF

0

10.38

Sigaro (cigar)

LF

0

10.88

Pomata (ointment)

LF

0

11.18

Sogliola (sole)

LF

0

10.97

Timone (tiller)

LF

0

11.05

Spigolo (edge)

LF

0

10.49

Vagone (carriage)

LF

16

10.75

Vedovo (widower)

LF

0

10.60

Velluto (velvet)

LF

16

10.86

Vipera (viper)

LF

14

10.83

Appendix B

Discriminant function analysis results

Lambda-Wilks: .205; approx. F(3. 10) = 12.88; p < .0009

Variables

Lambda

F

GDL 1

GDL 2

p-level

Word reading (correctness + speed standard scores)

.4136

17.01

1

12

.001

Percentage of recognized words

.3513

10.15

2

11

.003

Percentage of recognized graphemes

.205

12.88

3

10

.0009

Standardized coefficients for canonical variables

Variables

Root 1

    

Word reading (correctness + speed standard scores)

1.01

    

Percentage of recognized words

1.31

    

Percentage of recognized graphemes

−1.33

    

Classification matrix

Rows: Observed classifications

     

Columns: Predicted classifications

     

Group

Percent correct

G_1 p = .428

G_2 p = .571

  

G_1

83.33

5

1

  

G_2

100.00

0

8

  

Total

92.85

5

9

  

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Orsolini, M., Fanari, R., Cerracchio, S. et al. Phonological and lexical reading in Italian children with dyslexia. Read Writ 22, 933–954 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-008-9134-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-008-9134-x

Keywords

Navigation