Skip to main content
Log in

Crosslinguistic influence on phonological awareness for Korean–English bilingual children

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined (1) the potential influence of oral language characteristics of two languages that bilingual children acquire on their PA and (2) the relationship of PA in L1 with PA and literacy skills in L2, using data from Korean–English bilingual children. Thirty three Korean–English bilingual children, composed of two subsamples from two different locations/bilingual programs, participated in the study. The findings showed that the sample of bilingual children from two bilingual programs differed in their mean performances on intrasyllabic phonological awareness in Korean (i.e., rime awareness and body awareness). Furthermore, children’s Korean rime awareness, but not body awareness, was positively related to their phonological awareness and literacy skills in English. Finally, these children’s phonological awareness in Korean made a positive contribution to English decoding skills even after controlling for their English sight word reading skills. The results are discussed in light of interlingual influence on bilingual children’s phonological awareness.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The hierarchical structure of the syllable of this view is that the syllable is divided into body and coda. The body is composed of the onset and the nucleus.

  2. Saturday (or weekend) language schools provide opportunities for Korean-American children to learn the Korean language and literacy, and enhance ethnic cultural identity. These are found in many metropolitan areas across the United States. There were approximately 254 registered Korean weekend schools in the Los Angeles area in 2005 (Zhou & Kim, 2006).

  3. It is acknowledged that Fug is a real word. However, the children in this study would not have known the word.

  4. Since there is no single operational definition of English language learners, this statement is based on the broad definition of English Language Learners that all these children’s spoke a language other than English at home. It is acknowledged that English language learner status/classification is on a continuum and changing and temporary (see Francis, Rivera, Lesaux, Kieffer, & Rivera, 2006).

  5. The author wishes to thank an anonymous reviewer for this point.

References

  • August, D., Calderson, J. H., & Carlo, M. (2001, March/April). The transfer of skills from Spanish to English: A study of young learners. NABE News, pp. 11, 12, 42.

  • Bialystok, E., Majumder, S., & Martin, M. M. (2003). Developing phonological awareness: Is there a bilingual advantage? Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 27–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Branum-Martin, L., Mehta, P. D., Fletcher, J. M., Carlson, C. D., Ortiz, A., Carlo, M., et al. (2006). Bilingual PA: Multilevel construct validation among Spanish-speaking kindergarteners in transitional bilingual education classrooms. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 170–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caravolas, M., & Bruck, M. (1993). The effect of oral and written language input on children’s phonological awareness: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 55, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiappe, P., Glaser, B., & Ferko, D. (2007). Speech perception, vocabulary, and the development of reading skills in English among Korean- and English-speaking children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 154–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cho, J.-R., & McBride-Chang, C. (2005). Correlates of Korean Hangul acquisition among kindergartners and second graders. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 3–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cisero, C. A., & Royer, J. M. (1995). The development and cross-language transfer of PA. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 20, 275–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clay, M. M. (1979). The early detection of reading difficulties. London: Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Comeau, L., Cormier, P., Grandmaison, E., & Lacroix, D. (1999). A longitudinal study of phonological processing skills in children learning to read in a second language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 29–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, J. (1979). Linguistic interdependence and the educational development of bilingual children. Review of Educational Research, 49, 222–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, J. (1986). Empowering minority students: A framework for intervention. Harvard Educational Review, 56, 18–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Cara, B., & Goswami, U. (2002). Similarity relations among spoken words: The special status of rime in English. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 34, 416–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durgunoglu, A. Y., Nagy, W. E., & Hancin-Bhatt, B. J. (1993). Cross-language transfer of PA. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 453–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckman, F. R. (2004). From phonemic differences to constraint rankings: Research on second language phonology. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 513–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flege, J. E. (1995). Second language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. In W. Strange (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-language research (pp. 233–277). Baltimore: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Francis, D., Rivera, M., Lesaux, N., Kieffer, M., & Rivera, H. (2006). Practical guidelines for the education of English language learners: Research-based recommendations for instruction and academic interventions. Houston, TX: Center on Instruction English Language Learners Strand.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ganschow, L., & Sparks, R. (1995). Effects of direct instruction in Spanish phonology on the native-language skills and foreign-language aptitude of at-risk foreign-language learners. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28, 107–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (1991). Learning about spelling sequences: The role of onsets and rimes in analogies in reading. Child Development, 62, 1110–1123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., & Mead, F. (1992). Onset and rime awareness and analogies in reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 153–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kang, H., & Simpson, G. B. (1996). Development of semantic and phonological priming in a shallow orthography. Developmental Psychology, 32, 860–866.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S. (2007). Phonological awareness and literacy skills in Korean: An examination of the unique role of body-coda units. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 67–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S. Cat in a hat or cat in a cap? An investigation of developmental trajectories of phonological awareness for Korean children. Journal of Research in Reading (in press).

  • Lindsey, K. A., Mani, F. R., & Bailey, C. E. (2003). Prediction of first-grade reading in Spanish- speaking English-language learners. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 482–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metsala, J. L., & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In J. L. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89–120). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Navarra, J., Sebástian-Gallés, N., & Soto-Faraco, S. (2005). The perception of second language sounds in early bilinguals: New evidence from an implicit measure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 912–918.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quiroga, T., Lemos-Britton, Z., Mostafapour, E., Abbott, R. D., & Berninger, V. W. (2002). PA and beginning reading in Spanish-speaking ESL first graders: Research into practice. Journal of School Psychology, 40, 85–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stahl, S. A., & Murray, B. A. (1994). Defining PA and its relationship to early reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 221–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, I. (1980). The Korean writing system: An alphabet? A syllabary? A logography? In P. A. Kolers, M. E. Wrolstad, & H. Bouma (Eds.), Processing of visible language 2 (pp. 67–82). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., & Zukowski, A. (1991). Levels of PA. In S. Brady & D. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy (pp. 67–83). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological processing. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walley, A. C., Metsala, J. L., & Garlock, V. M. (2003). Spoken vocabulary growth: Its role in the development of phoneme awareness and early reading ability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 5–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, M., Park, Y., & Lee, K. R. (2006). Korean–English biliteracy acquisition: Cross-language phonological and orthographic transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 148–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yoon, H. K., Bolger, D. J., Kwon, O.-S., & Perfetti, C. A. (2002). Subysllabic units in reading. In L. Verhoeven, C. Elbro, & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (Vol. 11, pp. 139–163). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yoon, Y.-B., & Derwing, B. (2001). A language without a rhyme: Syllable structure experiments in Korean. Canadian Journal of Linguistics, 46, 187–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2006). Becoming literate in different languages: Similar problems, different solutions. Developmental Science, 9, 429–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, M., & Kim, S. S. (2006). Community forces, social capital, and educational achievement: The case of supplementary education in the Chinese and Korean immigrant communities. Harvard Educational Review, 71, 676–706.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The author would like to thank all the children, parents, and teachers in the study. In addition, the author wishes to thank anonymous reviewers for their constructive feedback.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Young-Suk Kim.

Appendices

Appendix A. Stimuli used in the Korean phonological awareness tasks

 

Blending task

Segmenting task

Syllable awareness

Practice item 1

토끼 /tokki/ [rabbit]

교회 /kyohwɛ/ [church]

Practice item 2

학교 /hakkyo/ [school]

가위 /kawi/ [sissors]

Item 1

수박 /subak/ [watermelon]

노래 /norɛ/ [songs]

Item 2

포도 /phodo/ [grapes]

머리 /mʌri/ [head/hair]

Item 3

비누 /pinu/ [soap]

바다 /pada/ [ocean]

Item 4

엄마 /ʌmma/ [mom]

케익 /kek] [cake]

Item 5

동생 /doŋsɛŋ/ [younger sibling]

공책/koŋʧɛk/ [notebook]

Body awareness

Practice item 1

강 /kaŋ/ [river]

밥 /pap/ [cooked rice]

Practice item 2

물 /mul/ [water]

병 /pyʌŋ/ [bottle]

Item 1

신 /ʃin/ [shoes]

창 /ʧaŋ/ [window]

Item 2

빵/ ppaŋ/ [bread]

별 /pyʌl/ [star]

Item 3

떡 /ttʌk/ [rice cake]

손 /son/ [hand]

Item 4

밥 /pap/ [cooked rice]

달 /tal/ [moon]

Item 5

말 /mal/ [horse]

집 /ʒip/ [house/home]

Rime awareness

Practice item 1

봄 /pom/ [spring]

방 /paŋ/ [room]

Practice item 2

펜 /phɛn/ [pen]

껌 /kkʌm/ [gum]

Item 1

산 /san/ [mountain]

공/koŋ/ [ball]

Item 2

목 /mok/ [neck]

문 /mun/ [door]

Item 3

밤 /pam/ [night/chestnut]

돈 /ton/ [money]

Item 4

책 /ʧ!ɛk/ [book]

팔 /phal/ [arms]

Item 5

눈 /nun/ [eyes]

국 /kuk/ [soup]

Phoneme awareness

Practice item 1

소 /so/ [cow]

자 /ʒa/ [ruler]

Practice item 2

나 /na/ [I/me]

표 /phyo/ [ticket]

Item 1

비 /pi/ [rain]

개 /kɛ/ [dog]

Item 2

새 /sɛ/ [bird]

초 /ʧo/ [candle]

Item 3

차 /ʧa/ [car/tea]

너 /nʌ/ [you]

Item 4

파 /pha/ [green onion]

코 /kho/ [nose]

Item 5

해 /hɛ/ [sun]

씨 /ssi/ [seed]

Practice item 3

잠 /ʒam/[sleep]

풀 /phul/ [grass]

Item 6

문 /mun/ [door]

곰 /kom/ [bear]

Item 7

감 /kam/ [persimmon]

발 /pal/ [foot]

Item 8

달 /tal/ [moon]

눈 /nun/ [eyes]

Item 9

땀 /ttam/ [sweat]

형 /hyʌŋ/ [older brother]

Item 10

김 /kim/ [laver]

공 /koŋ/ [ball]

Appendix B. Stimuli used for the Korean word reading

Practice item:

  1. 1.

    사자 /saʒa/ [lion]

Test items:

  1. 1.

    기차 /kiʧa/ [train]

  2. 2.

    물고기 /mulkkogi/ [fish]

  3. 3.

    산 /san/ [mountain]

  4. 4.

    집 /ʒip/ [house/home]

  5. 5.

    파도 /phado/ [waves]

  6. 6.

    나비 /nabi/ [butterfly]

  7. 7.

    라면 /lamyʌn/ [ramen noodles]

  8. 8.

    다리 /tari/ [legs]

  9. 9.

    코 /kho/ [nose]

  10. 10.

    하나 /hana/ [one]

  11. 11.

    바다 /pada/ [ocean]

  12. 12.

    엄마 /ʌmma/ [mom]

  13. 13.

    차 /ʧa/ [car/tea]

  14. 14.

    선생님 /sʌnsæŋnim/ [teacher]

  15. 15.

    토끼 /thokki/ [rabbit]

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kim, YS. Crosslinguistic influence on phonological awareness for Korean–English bilingual children. Read Writ 22, 843–861 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-008-9132-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-008-9132-z

Keywords

Navigation