Skip to main content
Log in

Understanding trustworthiness: using response latencies from CATI surveys to learn about the “crucial” variable in trust research

  • Published:
Quality & Quantity Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Research on trustworthiness and trust behavior so far has followed different methodological approaches and has generated conflicting evidence regarding their interrelationships. While several authors follow the definition of trustworthiness as a belief or a formed expectancy by Person A about Person B to do X (usually to reward trust), different hypotheses can be derived regarding its formation, depending on whether one treats trustworthiness as incentive-based or as a propensity or disposition. Additionally, distinct measurement approaches for trustworthiness exist, depending on the mode of data collection. With theoretical claims that trustworthiness represents “the crucial variable” (Hardin in Trust, 2006) for understanding and explaining successful cooperation based on trust, the article proposes the use of para-data in the form of response latency measurement to enhance the understanding of the thought processes behind forming an assessment of trustworthiness. The study uses pooled data from two CATI surveys conducted in Germany in 2012 to test hypotheses on the underlying cognitive process of forming an expectation of trustworthiness by applying the techniques of both response latency measurement and Cox regression models. We find that the applied survey measures of trustworthiness generate inconsistent results regarding the underlying process of forming expectations. Consequences for future research are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. This conceptualization of trustworthiness has led to an agreement within research on trust and trustworthiness that equates the expectation of trustworthiness with trust (Glaeser et al. 2000; although this may not be entirely warranted, see Hardin 2006, p. 32).

  2. Freitag and Traunmüller (2009, p. 9) distinguish dispositions from experiences to understand the logic of trust, but they differentiate with regards to the two facets of particularized and generalized trust. We will limit our study to the investigation of trustworthiness as we will not be able to account for trusting behavior during telephone interviews. We return to this point later.

  3. It appears obvious that the assumption that expected trustworthiness can be equated with trust is misguided, as it ignores the conditions of an action based on trust that is shaped by G and L as well p. Expectancies can be formed without trusting. We cannot elaborate on that further, see Hardin (2006) on these issues.

  4. We put the player in quotation marks as we did not perform any real experiments or trust games. The respondents in the survey were merely put in two situations resembling their role in games, although there did not exist any dyadic interactions. Respondents answered question from the perspective of “Player A” and “Player B”, or the trustor and the trustee of the traditional trust game, respectively. Details follow in the section on methods and data.

  5. Obviously, this relates well to the ideas of rational choice theory, where actions are explained by variation of conditions given a set of preferences, levels of information and assumptions about the rationality of actors (Opp 1999).

  6. Prior experiences are considered a crucial predictor to explain action based on trust (Fehr et al. 2002, p. 540).

  7. Scholars that appear to have replicated this finding do not speak about trustworthiness but rather about reciprocity, risk aversion or unconditional kindness (Altmann et al. 2008; Ashraf et al. 2006).

  8. Usually the amount sent is doubled or tripled and the trustee decides about the amount to be sent back. Ashraf, Bohnet, and Piankov (2006, p. 197) go so far to define trustworthiness by this measurement.

  9. To clarify, we do not accept the proposal “that trust is the expectation about other people’s behavior” and that “both the answers to the WVS-question and the sender’s expectation in a traditional trust game can be used as a measure” for trust (Sapienza et al. 2013, p. 1330). Trust represents an action based on an expectation.

  10. These efforts go beyond the usual practice of using response time measurement for pretests of questionnaires to detect mode effects or undesired response tendencies (for an overview see Yan and Tourangeau 2008; Dillman et al. 2009).

  11. See Neumann (2014) for additional validity checks of latency measures.

  12. We will document results for both valid and invalid measurements as an additional feature to our study.

  13. As RLD or RDD procedures are not feasible in Germany, the Gabler–Haeder design draws household numbers randomly from pre-generated blocks of entries. For more details, see Gabler and Häder (2002) for Germany and Häder (2012) for Europe.

  14. This has to be considered rather low with regards to average response rates for telephone interviews in Germany (Häder 2012, pp. 124–125), but Wang et al. (2014) report equally low response rates for the US.

  15. Three cases were dropped from the sample because of ineligibility due to age.

  16. Descriptive statistics for the Cox models are generated by applying the stset-command in Stata. These summary statistics can be provided upon request. Note that for all these models, the proportionality assumption holds.

  17. Cronbach’s \(\alpha \) for the trust index comes in at only 0.63, which has to be considered rather low. To circumvent the low reliability of the trust index, analyses were performed with single answers to the generalized trust item, but results did not change at all. Result can be provided upon request from the authors.

  18. These findings are not in line with the results in Mulligan et al. (2003, p. 297), who generate similar results for measurements with and without validity coding.

  19. With respect to the skewed distribution of the dependent variable, we also estimated two-tier regression models via the craggit-command in Stata (Cragg 1971; Burke 2009). Results do not differ in the sense that, for the first stage, answers with the highest level of behavioral trustworthiness were given fast and prior experience mattered, whereas generalized trust could not be considered as a relevant predictor. The coefficients for socio-demographic variables showed consistent results within the first stage model. Same holds for the part of the truncated regression model, although the latency scores lose their predictive power for the remaining levels or reported behavioral trustworthiness (measurements were validated, \(n=239\)). Results are not shown, but can be obtained upon request.

  20. The results could be criticized regarding the potential confounding effect of generalized trust on the dependent variable of expected trustworthiness. Both variables correlate at 0.22 \((p<0.05, n=569)\), which can be considered rather low. Dropping the generalized trust variable from the model yields the same results, with both indices for the response latencies as the only variables that explain the variation of the responses to the trustworthiness item T2. Results can be provided upon request.

  21. Notable exemptions exist: For instance, Przepiorka and Diekmann (2013, p. 3) test a model proposed by Posner (1998) by an experimental sequence where the signal of trustworthiness is sent by a trustee prior to the placement of trust by Player A, which conforms to the causal structure of a trust relationship.

References

  • Altmann, S., Dohmen, T., Wibral, M.: Do the reciprocal trust less? Econ. Lett. 99(3), 454–457 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashraf, N., Bohnet, I., Piankov, N.: Decomposing trust and trustworthiness. Exp. Econ. 9(3), 193–208 (2006)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bassili, J.N.: Response latency versus certainty as indexes of the strength of voting intentions in a CATI survey. Public Opin. Quart. 57(1), 54–61 (1993)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bassili, J.N., Krosnick, J.A.: Do strength-related attitude properties determine susceptibility to response effects? New evidence from response latency, attitude extremity, and aggregate indices. Polit. Psychol. 21(1), 107–132 (2000)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becerra, M., Gupta, A.K.: Perceived trustworthiness within the organization: the moderating impact of communication frequency on trustor and trustee effects. Org. Sci. 14(1), 32–44 (2003)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Ner, A., Halldorsson, F.: Trusting and trustworthiness: what are they, how to measure them, and what affects them. J. Econ. Psychol. 31(1), 64–79 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., McCabe, K.: Trust, reciprocity, and social history. Games Econ. Behav. 10(1), 122–142 (1995)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beugelsdijk, S.: A note on the theory and measurement of trust in explaining differences in economic growth. Camb. J. Econ. 30, 371–387 (2006)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bicchieri, C.: Norms, preferences, and conditional behavior. Polit. Philos. Econ. 9, 297–313 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burke, W.J.: Fitting and interpreting Cragg’s tobit alternative using Stata. Stata J. 9(4), 584–592 (2009)

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D.T., Fiske, D.W.: Convergent and discriminant validity by the mutitrait- multimethod matrix. Psychol. Bull. 56(2), 81–105 (1959)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chaiken, S., Trope, Y. (eds.): Dual-Process Theories in Social Psychology. Guileford Press, New York (1999)

  • Charness, G., Haruvy, E., Sonsino, D.: Social distance and reciprocity: an Internet experiment. J. Econ. Behav. Org. 63(1), 88–103 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, J.L.: Foundations of Social Theory. Belknap Press, Cambridge (1990)

    Google Scholar 

  • Colquitt, J.A., Scott, B.A., LePine, J.A.: Trust, trustworthiness, and trust propensity: a meta-analytic test of their unique relationships with risk taking and job performance. J. Appl. Psychol. 92(4), 909–927 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cragg, J.G.: Some Statistical models for limited dependent variables with application to the demand for durable goods. Econometrica 39(5), 829–844 (1971)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delhey, J., Newton, K., Welzel, C.: How general is trust in “most peopleT? Solving the radius of trust problem. Am. Sociol. Rev. 76(5), 786–807 (2011)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dillman, D.A., et al.: Response rate and measurement differences in mixed-mode surveys using mail, telephone, interactive voice response (IVR) and the Internet. Soc. Sci. Res. 38(1), 1–18 (2009)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dufwenberg, M., Gneezy, U.: Measuring beliefs in an experimental lost wallet game. Games Econ. Behav. 30(2), 163–182 (2000)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A.H., Chaiken, S.: The Psychology of Attitudes. Harcourt Brace, Fort Worth (1993)

    Google Scholar 

  • Engell, A.D., Haxby, J.V., Todorov, A.: Implicit trustworthiness decisions: automatic coding of face properties in the human amygdala. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 19(9), 1508–1519 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Esser, H.: The definition of the situation. Kölner Zeitschrift für Soziologie und Sozialpsychologie 48(1), 1–34 (1996)

    Google Scholar 

  • Etang, A., Fielding, D., Knowles, S.: Are survey measures of trust correlated with experimental trust? Evidence from Cameroon. J. Dev. Stud. 48(12), 1813–1827 (2012)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Faust, M., et al.: Individual differences in information-processing rate and amount: implications for group differences in response latency. Psychol. Bull. 125(6), 777–799 (1999)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R.H.: A practical guide to the use of response latency in social psychological research. In: Hendrick, C., Clark, M.S. (eds.) Review of Personality and Social Psychology—Research Methods in Personality and Social Psychology, pp. 74–97. Sage Publications, Newbury Park (1990)

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R.H., et al.: On the automatic activation of attitudes. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 50(2), 229–238 (1986)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R.H.: Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: The MODE model as an integrative framework. In: Zanna, M.P. (ed.) Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, pp. 75–109. Academic Press, San Diego (1990)

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R.H.: Attitudes as object-evaluation associations: determinants, consequences, and correlates of attitude accessibility. In: Petty, R.E., Krosnick, J.A. (eds.) Attitude Strength: Antecedents and Consequences, pp. 247–282. Erlbaum, Mahwah (1995)

    Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., et al.: A nation-wide laboratory—examining trust and trustworthiness by integrating behavioral experiments into representative surveys. Schmollers Jahrbuch 122(4), 519–542 (2002)

    Google Scholar 

  • Freitag, M., Traunmüller, R.: Spheres of trust: an empirical analysis of the foundations of particularised and generalised trust. Eur. J. Polit. Res. 48(6), 782–803 (2009)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gabler, S., Häder, S.: Telefonstichproben. Methodische Innovationen und Anwendungen in Deutschland, Münster. Waxman Verlag, New York (2002)

    Google Scholar 

  • Gambetta, D.: Can we trust trust? In: Gambetta, D. (ed.) Trust: Making and Breaking Cooperative Relations, pp. 213–237. Basil Blackwell, Cambridge (1988)

    Google Scholar 

  • Glaeser, E., et al.: Measuring trust. Quart. J. Econ. 115(3), 811–846 (2000)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glanville, J.L., Paxton, P.: How do we learn to trust? A confirmatory tetrad analysis of the sources of generalized trust. Soc. Psychol. Quart. 70(3), 230–242 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graeff, P., Svendsen, G.T.: Trust and corruption: the influence of positive and negative social capital on the economic development in the European Union. Qual. Quant. 47(5), 2829–2846 (2013)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grant, J.T., Mockabee, S.T., Monson, J.Q.: Campaign effects on the accessibility of party identification. Polit. Res. Quart. 63(4), 811–821 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hardin, R.: Trust. Polity Press, Cambridge (2006)

    Google Scholar 

  • Häder, S.H.: Telephone Surveys in Europe/Research and Practice, 1st edn. Springer, Berlin (2012)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Holtgraves, T.: Social desirability and self-reports: testing models of socially desirable responding. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 30(2), 161–172 (2004)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huckfeldt, R., Sprague, J.: Political consequences of inconsistency: the accessibility and stability of abortion attitudes. Polit. Psychol. 21(1), 57–79 (2000)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaminska, O., Foulsham, T.: Understanding sources of social desirability bias in different modes: evidence from eye-tracking. ISERWorking Paper Ser. 13, 1–11 (2013)

    Google Scholar 

  • Karlan, D.S.: Using experimental economics to measure social capital and predict financial decisions. Am. Econ. Rev. 95(5), 1688–1699 (2005)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koopmans, R., Veit, S.: Cooperation in ethnically diverse neighborhoods: a lost-letter experiment. Polit. Psychol. 35(3), 379–400 (2014)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, R.M.: Trust and distrust in organizations: emerging perspectives, enduring questions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 50, 569–598 (1999)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krumpal, I.: Determinants of social desirability bias in sensitive surveys: a literature review. Qual. Quant. 47(4), 2025–2047 (2013)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, J.D., Weigert, A.: Trust as a social reality. Soc. Forces 63(4), 967–985 (1985)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R.C., Davis, J.H., Schoorman, F.D.: An integrative model of organizational trust. Acad. Manag. Rev. 20(3), 709–734 (1995)

    Google Scholar 

  • Messick, D.M., Kramer, R.M.: Trust as a form of shallow morality. In: Cook, K.S. (ed.) Trust in Society, pp. 89–117. Russell Sage Foundation, New York (2001)

    Google Scholar 

  • Milgram, S., Mann, L., Harter, S.: The lost-letter technique: a tool of social research. Public Opin. Quart. 29(3), 437–438 (1965)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mulligan, K., et al.: Response latency methodology for survey research: measurement and modeling strategies. Polit. Anal. 11(3), 289–301 (2003)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nannestad, P.: What have we learned about generalized trust, if anything? Annu. Rev. Polit. Sci. 11(1), 413–436 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, R.: the validity of response latency measurement In CATI surveys. Prepared for XVIII. In: ISA World Congress 13–19 July 2014 Yokohama (2014)

  • Opp, K.-D.: Contending conceptions of the theory of rational action. J. Theo. Polit. 11(2), 171–202 (1999)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orbell, J., Dawes, R.M.: A “cognitive miser” theory of cooperators’ advantage. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 85(2), 515–528 (1991)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, E.A.: Symbols, signals, and social norms in politics and law. J. Legal Stud. 27(2), 765–798 (1998)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Przepiorka, W., Diekmann, A.: Temporal embeddedness and signals of trustworthiness: experimental tests of a game theoretic model in the United Kingdom, Russia, and Switzerland. Eur. Sociol. Rev. 29(5), 1010–1023 (2013)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rotter, J.B.: Interpersonal-trust, trustworthiness, and gullibility. Am. Psychol. 35(1), 1–7 (1980)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sapienza, P., Toldra-Simats, A., Zingales, L.: Understanding trust. Econ. J. 123(573), 1313–1332 (2013)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schaeffer, N.C.: Asking questions about threatening topics: a selective overview. In: Stone, A.A., et al. (eds.) The Science of Self—Report: Implications for Research and Practice. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, London (2000)

    Google Scholar 

  • Sønderskov, K.M.: Explaining large-N cooperation: generalized social trust and the social exchange heuristic. Ration. Soc. 23(1), 51–74 (2011)

  • Stocké, V.: Attitudes toward surveys, attitude accessibility and the effect on respondents ísusceptibility to nonresponse. Qual. Quant. 40(2), 259–288 (2006)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F., Deutsch, R.: Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 8(3), 220–247 (2004)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sturgis, P., Smith, P.: Assessing the validity of generalized trust questions: what kind of trust are we measuring? Int. J. Public Opin. Res. 22(1), 74–92 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Urban, D., Mayerl, J.: Response latency measurement in survey-based behaviour research. Kölner Zeitschrift für Soziologie und Sozialpsychologie 59(4), 692–713 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uslaner, E.M.: Democracy and social capital. In: Warren, M.E. (ed.) Democracy and Trust. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1999)

    Google Scholar 

  • Uslaner, E.M.: The Moral Foundations of Trust. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (2002)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Uslaner, E.M.: The foundations of trust: macro and micro. Camb. J. Econ. 32(2), 289–294 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uslaner, E.M.: Trust and corruption revisited: how and why trust and corruption shape each other. Qual. Quant. 47(6), 3603–3608 (2013)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, W., Rothschild, D., Goel,S., Gelman, A.: Forecasting elections with non-representative polls. Int. J. Forecast. (2014). doi:10.1016/j.ijforecast.2014.06.001

  • Williamson, O.E.: Calculativeness, trust, and economic organization. J. Law Econ. 34, 453–502 (1993)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winston, J.S., et al.: Automatic and intentional brain responses during evaluation of trustworthiness of faces. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 277–283 (2002)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yamagishi, T.: Trust as a form of social intelligence. In: Cook, K.S. (ed.) Trust in Society, pp. 119–149. Russell Sage Foundation, New York (2001)

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamagishi, T., Yamagishi, M.: Trust and commitment in the United States and Japan. Motiv. Emot. 18, 129–166 (1994)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yan, T., Tourangeau, R.: Fast times and easy questions: the effects of age, experience and question complexity on web survey response times. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 22(1), 51–68 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank participants of the RC45 and RC33 sessions of ISA World Congress 2014 in Yokohama, Japan, and the anonymous referees for helpful comments. The author is indebted to Lars Hiller for technical support.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robert Neumann.

Appendix

Appendix

See Tables 6, 7 and 8.

Table 6 Summary statistics for respondent’s age, generalized trust and the two latency measures (pooled from two sub-samples)
Table 7 Frequency statistics for the independent variables (pooled from two sub-samples)
Table 8 Frequency statistics for the validation coding of the manual response time measurement for the items T1 (behavioral trustworthiness) and T2 (expected trustworthiness), pooled from two sub-samples

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Neumann, R. Understanding trustworthiness: using response latencies from CATI surveys to learn about the “crucial” variable in trust research. Qual Quant 50, 43–64 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-014-0136-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-014-0136-2

Keywords

Navigation