Skip to main content
Log in

Assessing the Predictive Value of Fertility Expectations Through a Cognitive–Social Model

  • Published:
Population Research and Policy Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper grounds its analysis in a novel model (Bachrach and Morgan in Popul Dev Rev, 39:459–485, 2013) that suggests that responses to questions about fertility intentions may reflect distinct phenomena at distinct points in the life course. The model suggests that women form "true" intentions when their circumstances make the issue of childbearing salient and urgent enough to draw the cognitive resources needed to make a conscious plan; before this, women report intentions based on cognitive images of family and self. We test the implications of this model for reported fertility expectations using NLSY79 data that measure expectations throughout the life course. We find that early in the life course, before marriage and parenthood, women’s fertility expectations are associated with family background and cognitive images of family and future self. Later in the life course, as women experience life course transitions that confer statuses normatively associated with childbearing—such as marriage—and parenthood itself, their reported expectations are better predictors of their fertility than before they passed these life course milestones. Our empirical results provide support for a model which has important implications for both the measurement and conceptualization of women’s intended and expected fertility.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. An earlier version of this theory, the Theory of Reasoned Action, was proposed by Fishbein and Ajzen in 1975.

  2. For example, the international Generations and Gender Programme, begun in 2004, extensively draws on the TPB in its conceptualization and in the operationalization of its measurement instruments (Vikat et al. 2007). A 2010 conference focused entirely on TPB as a model for studying fertility behavior. http://www.oeaw.ac.at/vid/in2b/.

  3. An exception is Miller (1994) who has advanced a traits-desires-intentions-behavior or T-D-I-B framework that outlines the sequence of motivational dispositions and conscious states that lead humans to behave to have or avoid having children.

  4. Even the TPB’s author, Icek Ajzen, challenged demographers to modify the theory to more accurately describe fertility during the 2010 conference (Morgan and Bachrach 2011).

  5. The Theory of Planned Behavior (Fishbein and Ajzen 2010) also views intentions as shaped by attitudes and norms, but does not specify how intentions may develop over the life course.

  6. In the cognitive–social model, a person’s experience of different structures (as influenced by family background, education, religion, ethnicity, etc.) also shapes the representations and emotional meanings of self and family life constructed in the brain, creating another path for influencing fertility intentions.

  7. Demographers have tended to treat intentions and expectations as interchangeable, and indeed, evidence suggests that when these questions are posed to survey respondents, they tend to elicit very similar answers (Morgan 2001; Ryder and Westoff 1965).

  8. For the reasoning behind this, see Bachrach (2014).

  9. As discussed in the introduction, Morgan and Rackin (2010) addressed a related question, but did not examine changes in the accuracy of expectations across life course transitions.

  10. Final parity was the total number of children reported in the last available wave after the 40th birthday.

  11. The transition to completing education was not as crisply defined as the transition to marriage and first birth (the dates of first marriage and birth were recorded) because women may return to education as their life course proceeds, and this transition may be triggered by the occurrence or nonoccurrence of marriage and birth. We have explored other measures of completion of education and find generally similar results.

  12. First cohabitation was based on the household roster and measured the first time a respondent reported living with a person of the opposite sex who was not their spouse. The first cohabitation, however, may have not been recorded because it occurred before the first survey or between waves. Nevertheless, this is the only measure available across all waves.

  13. Of women in our sample, 68 % had information in every interval, 16 % were missing information in one interval, 11 % were missing information in two intervals, and the remaining 6 % were missing in more than two intervals. The number of years between each interval range between 1 and 3 years with the majority being two years (only the 1979–1982 interval was three years and the 1982–1986 intervals were 1 year).

References

  • Aiken, A. R., & Potter, J. E. (2013). Are Latina women ambivalent about pregnancies they are trying to prevent? Evidence from the Border Contraceptive Access Study. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 45, 196–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I. (2011). Reflections on Morgan and Bachrach’s critique. Vienna Yearbook of Population Research, 9, 63–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderton, D. L., Tsuya, N. O., Bean, L. L., & Mineau, G. P. (1987). Intergenerational transmission of relative fertility and life course patterns. Demography, 24, 467–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Axinn, W. G., Clarkberg, M. E., & Thornton, A. (1994). Family influences on family size preferences. Demography, 31, 65–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Axinn, W. G., & Thornton, A. (1996). The influence of parents’ marital dissolutions on children’s attitudes toward family formation. Demography, 33, 66–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bachrach, C. (2014). Culture and demography: From reluctant bedfellows to committed partners. Demography, 51, 3–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bachrach, C. A., & Morgan, S. P. (2013). A cognitive-social model of fertility intentions. Population and Development Review, 39, 459–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barber, J. S. (2001). Ideational influences on the transition to parenthood: Attitudes toward childbearing and competing alternatives. Social Psychology Quarterly, 64(2), 101–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barber, J. S. (2011). The Theory of Planned Behaviour: Considering drives, proximity and dynamics. Vienna Yearbook of Population Research, 9, 31–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, G., & Wellings, K. (2002). What is a ‘planned’ pregnancy? Empirical data from a British study. Social Science and Medicine, 55, 545–557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bongaarts, J. (2001). Fertility and reproductive preferences in post-transitional societies. Population and Development Review, 27, 260–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borrero, S., Nikolajski, C., Steinberg, J. R., Freedman, L., Akers, A. Y., Ibrahim, S., & Schwarz, E. B. (2015). “It just happens”: A qualitative study exploring low-income women’s perspectives on pregnancy intention and planning. Contraception, 91, 150–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bumpass, L., & Lu, H.-H. (2000). Trends in cohabitation and implications for children s family contexts in the United States. Population Studies, 54, 29–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, A. R., & Beach, I. R. (1981). Error patterns in the prediction of fertility behavior1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 11, 475–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, O. D., Freedman, R., Coble, J. M., & Slesinger, D. P. (1965). Marital fertility and size of family of orientation. Demography, 2, 508–515.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finer, L. B., & Henshaw, S. K. (2006). Disparities in rates of unintended pregnancy in the United States, 1994 and 2001. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 38, 90–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, R. C., Stanford, J. B., Jameson, P., & DeWitt, M. J. (1999). Exploring the concepts of intended, planned, and wanted pregnancy. The Journal of Family Practice, 48, 117–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (2010). Predicting and changing behavior. New York: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayford, S. R., & Morgan, S. P. (2008). Religiosity and fertility in the United States: The role of fertility intentions. Social Forces, 86, 1163–1188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heaton, T. B., Jacobson, C. K. & Holland, K. (1999). Persistence and change in decisions to remain childless. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61(2), 531–539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hendershot, G. E. (1969). Familial satisfaction, birth order, and fertility values. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 27–33.

  • Johnson, B. E., & Freymeyer, R. H. (1989). Replicating family size: Does living in a single parent family matter? Sociological Focus, 22, 263–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, N. E., & Stokes, C. S. (1976). Family size in successive generations: The effects of birth order, intergenerational change in lifestyle, and familial satisfaction. Demography, 13, 175–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Hanks, J., Bachrach, C., Morgan, S. P., & Kohler, H.-P. (2011). Understanding family change and variation: Structure, conjuncture, and action. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Malle, B. F., Moses, L. J. & Baldwin, D. A. (2001). Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social cognition: MIT Press: Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, W. B. (1994). Childbearing motivations, desires, and intentions: A theoretical framework. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 120(2), 223–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, W. B. (2011). Differences between fertility desires and intentions: Implications for theory, research and policy. Vienna Yearbook of Population Research, 9, 75–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Monnier, A. (1989). Fertility intentions and actual behaviour. A longitudinal study: 1974, 1976, 1979. Population English Selection No. 1, 44(1), 237–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, S. P. (2001) Should fertility intentions inform fertility forecasts. In Proceedings of US Census Bureau Conference “The direction of fertility in the United States”, Washington, DC: US Census Bureau.

  • Morgan, S. P. & Bachrach, C. A. (2011). Is the Theory of Planned Behaviour an appropriate model for human fertility? Vienna Yearbook of Population Research, 9, 11–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, S. P., & Rackin, H. (2010). The correspondence between fertility intentions and behavior in the United States. Population and Development Review, 36, 91–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ní Bhrolcháin, M. & Beaujouan, É. (2011). Uncertainty in fertility intentions in Britain, 1979–2007. Vienna Yearbook of Population Research, 9, 99–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ní Bhrolcháin, M. & Beaujouan, E. (2012). How real are reproductive goals? Uncertainty and the construction of fertility preferences. San Francisco, CA: Population Association of America.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noack, T. & Østby, L. (2002). Free to choose—but unable to stick to it? Norwegian fertility expectations and subsequent behaviour in the following 20 years. Dynamics of Fertility and Partnership in Europe. Insights and Lessons from Comparative Research, 2, 103–116.

  • Pagnini, D. L. & Rindfuss, R. R. (1993). The divorce of marriage and childbearing: Changing attitudes and behavior in the United States. Population and Development Review, 19(2), 331–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pearce, L. D. (2002). The influence of early life course religious exposure on young adults’ dispositions toward childbearing. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 41, 325–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quesnel-Vallée, A., & Morgan, S. P. (2003). Missing the target? Correspondence of fertility intentions and behavior in the US. Population Research and Policy Review, 22, 497–525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryder, N. B., & Westoff, C. F. (1965). Reproduction in the United States. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sable, M. R., & Libbus, M. K. (2000). Pregnancy intention and pregnancy happiness: Are they different? Maternal and Child Health Journal, 4, 191–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schoen, R., Astone, N. M., Kim, Y. J., Nathanson, C. A. & Fields, J. M. (1999). Do fertility intentions affect fertility behavior? Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61(3), 790–799.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simons, J. (1978). Illusions about attitudes. Population Decline in Europe: Implications of a Declining Stationary Population (pp. 197–214). London: Edward Arnold Publishers, Council of Europe.

  • Trussell, J., Vaughan, B. & Stanford, J. (1999). Are all contraceptive failures unintended pregnancies? Evidence from the 1995 National Survey of Family Growth. Family Planning Perspectives, 246–260.

  • Van de Giessen, H., Bell, W., Szabo, K., Larson, A., Mitra, S., Sathar, Z., et al. (1992). Using birth expectations information in national population forecasts. International Family Planning Perspectives, 18, 223–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vikat, A., Spéder, Z., Beets, G., Billari, F., Bühler, C., Desesquelles, A., Fokkema, T., Hoem, J. M., MacDonald, A. L., Neyer, G. R., Pailhé, A., Pinnelli, A. & Solaz, A. (2007). Generations and gender survey (GGS): Towards a better understanding of relationships and processes in the life course. Demographic research, 17, 389–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waite, L. J. & Stolzenberg, R. M. (1976). Intended childbearing and labor force participation of young women: Insights from nonrecursive models. American Sociological Review, 41(2), 235–252.

  • Wertheimer, R. F., Long, M. & Vandivere, S. (2001) Welfare recipients’ attitudes toward welfare, nonmarital childbearing, and work: Implications for reform? New Federalism National Survey of America’s Families. Urban Institute.

  • Westoff, C. F., Mishler, E. G. & Kelly, E. L. (1957). Preferences in size of family and eventual fertility twenty years after. American Journal of Sociology, 491–497.

  • Westoff, C. F., & Ryder, N. B. (1977). The predictive validity of reproductive intentions. Demography, 14, 431–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zagorsky, J., & White, L. (1999). NLSY79 user’s guide: A guide to the 1979–1998 National Longitudinal Survey of Youth data. Washington, DC: US Department of Labor.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmer, B. G. & Fulton, J. (1980). Size of family, life chances, and reproductive behavior. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 42(3), 657–670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Heather M. Rackin.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rackin, H.M., Bachrach, C.A. Assessing the Predictive Value of Fertility Expectations Through a Cognitive–Social Model. Popul Res Policy Rev 35, 527–551 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11113-016-9395-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11113-016-9395-z

Keywords

Navigation