Skip to main content
Log in

Listening to the Government: How Information Shapes Responsibility Attributions

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Assigning credit and blame in systems of multilevel government, such as federal states, requires information. This paper examines how voters respond to information about policy outcomes when attributing responsibility to multiple levels of government in a European context. Using an experimental design, we show that the responsibility attributions of British voters are affected by perceptual biases, notably their feelings about the government and the European Union (EU). But interestingly, we also find that voters, regardless of their predispositions, are only responsive to information they receive from their national government, whereas they ignore information provided by EU officials. These findings have implications not only for our understanding of attribution in systems of multiple levels of government, but also for how voters use information selectively depending on the credibility of the source.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The same applies if we describe the Union’s executive as a dual executive consisting of both the Commission and the Council. The Council plays both a legislative and an executive role in the EU and is made up by members of national governments of different partisan persuasions.

  2. Notably utilitarian cost-benefit calculations concerned with whether individuals stand to gain from market integration have been shown to influence citizens’ support for the EU (see Gabel 1998; McLaren 2006).

  3. The articles that we created were based on existing stories from national newspapers and pre-tested prior to their introduction in the lab using focus groups. The focus groups reported that they thought that the news articles were, in fact, real news stories from a legitimate source. The news stories resembled in format material from the BBC website, which is one of the most widely used online news sources in the UK.

  4. See Mintz et al. (2006) for evidence against using students and Druckman and Kam (2011) for evidence in favour of using students.

  5. Participants had an average age of 29, but included people from 18 to 72. Slightly more women (59%) than men took part in the experiment. 36% of the participants were still in education, and among those no longer in education, the majority held some type of university degree (58%), making our sample considerably more educated than the British electorate. Our participants were also more middle class than the electorate, with around half of employed people working in professional or managerial jobs.

  6. The participants were informed that they were participating in a research study about contemporary issues in politics and the media. The general instructions informed participants that they would have to complete two questionnaires and read two news articles.

  7. Conventional measures of partisanship would give us only around a quarter of the sample as government supporters; by using a 0–10 scale of government approval we are able to get a more nuanced guide to any individual’s view of the government party. It also means that we have an analogous measure to the 0–10 EU support scale.

  8. For example, if a participant was given a positive report about the economic downtown and the source was the government, then the same positive direction of information and same government source were carried over to the status of healthcare report.

  9. Note that our subsequent analysis excludes the people (eight for the EU source treatment and eight for the government source treatment) who were unable to identify whether the treatment news article they received contained positive or negative information on the economy, and in total seven people (five for the EU source treatment and two for the government source treatment) that were unable to identify whether the treatment news article they received contained positive or negative information about healthcare. This is designed to eliminate subjects who simply did not pay any attention to the articles, and uses the question ‘In your opinion, did the first article have a positive, negative, or neutral assessment of the economic situation in Britain? (Positive, Neutral, Negative, Don’t know).

  10. These questions are designed to focus on ‘functional responsibility’ (or what some social psychologists call ‘role responsibility’), which refers to the obligations that institutions or individuals are expected to fulfil. It is worth noting that we are not ‘cueing’ participants in any way by asking them to directly credit or blame anybody for good or bad conditions, but rather asking for a more ‘objective’ assessment of responsibility. This makes any estimates of perceptual bias effects more conservative.

  11. See Consolidated versions of the Treaty on European Union and the Treaty on the Functioning of the European Union, Title 1, Articles 2–6 (Official Journal C 115 of 9 May 2008).

  12. Given EU support and (Labour) government support were potentially correlated, we also ran models predicting EU attributions of responsibility which additionally included the government support variable and an interaction between this and treatment. These interactions were not statistically significant, and the results we present here were not affected.

References

  • Anderson, C. J. (2000). Economic voting and political context: a comparative perspective. Electoral Studies, 19(2–3), 151–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, C. (2006). Economic voting and multilevel governance: A comparative individual-level analysis. American Journal of Political Science, 50, 446–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arceneaux, K. (2006). The federal face of voting: Are elected officials held accountable for the functions relevant to their office? Political Psychology, 27, 731–745.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atkeson, L. R., & Partin, R. W. (1995). Economic and referendum voting: A comparison of gubernatorial and senatorial elections. American Political Science Review, 89(1), 99–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atkeson, L. R., & Partin, R. W. (1998). Economic and referendum voting and the problem of data choice: A reply. American Journal of Political Science, 42(3), 1003–1007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bovens, M. (1998). The quest for responsibility: Accountability and citizenship in complex organizations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, M. B., & Brown, R. J. (1998). Intergroup relations. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology, Vol. 2 (4th ed., pp. 554–594). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, A., Converse, P., Miller, W., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American voter. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (2002). Undivided loyalties: Is national identity an obstacle to European integration? European Union Politics, 3(4), 387–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, F. (2004). Government responsibility and electoral accountability in federations. Publius, 34, 19–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, F. (2008). Whodunnit? Voters and responsibility in Canadian federalism. Canadian Journal of Political Science, 41, 627–654.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Vries, C. E. (2007). Sleeping giant: Fact or fairytale? How European integration affects national elections. European Union Politics, 8(3), 363–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Domke, D., Lagos, T., Lapointe, M., Meade, M., & Xenos, M. (2000). Elite messages and source cues: Moving beyond partisanship. Political Communication, 17(4), 395–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N. (2001a). On the limits of framing effects: Who can frame? Journal of Politics, 63(4), 1041–1066.

    Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N. (2001b). The implications of framing effects for citizen competence. Political Behavior, 23(3), 225–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N., & Kam, C. (2011). Students as experimental participants: A defense of the “narrow data base”. In J. N. Druckman, D. P. Green, J. H. Kuklinski, & A. Lupia (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of experimental political science. Boston: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • EES (2009). European parliament election study 2009, Voter Study, Advance Release, 01/05/2010, (www.piredeu.eu).

  • Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (2007). Social cognition. From brains to culture. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Føllesdal, A., & Hix, S. (2006). Why there is a democratic deficit in the EU: A response to Majone and Moravcsik. Journal of Common Market Studies, 44(3), 533–562.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gabel, M. J. (1998). Interest and integration. Market liberalization, public opinion and European Union. Ann Arbor, MI: The University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gomez, B. T., & Wilson, J. M. (2008). Political sophistication and attributions of blame in the wake of Hurricane Katrina. Publius, 38(4), 633–650.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hellwig, T. (2001). Interdependence, government constraints, and economic voting. Journal of Politics, 63, 1141–1162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewstone, M. (1989). Causal attribution: From cognitive processes to collective beliefs. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hooghe, L., & Marks, G. (2003). Unraveling the Central state, but how? Types of multi-level governance. American Political Science Review, 97(2), 233–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hooghe, L., & Marks, G. (2004). Does identity or economic rationality drive public opinion on european integration? PS: Political Science and Politics, 37, 415–420.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hooghe, L., & Marks, G. (2009). A postfunctionalist theory of European integration: From permissive consensus to constraining dissensus. British Journal of Political Science, 39(1), 1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hovland, C., & Weiss, W. (1951). The influence of source credibility on communication effectiveness. Public Opinion Quarterly, 15, 635–650.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hovland, C., Janis, I., & Kelley, H. (1953). Communication and persuasion. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johns, R. (2010). Credit where it’s due? Valence politics, attributions of responsibility, and multi-level elections. Political Behavior, 33(1), 53–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Key, V. O. (1966). The responsible electorate. New York: Vintage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohler-Koch, B. (1996). Catching up with change: The transformation of governance in the European Union. Journal of European Public Policy, 3(3), 359–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, R. R., & Redlawsk, D. P. (2001). Advantages and disadvantages of cognitive heuristics in political decision making. American Journal of Political Science, 45(4), 951–971.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • León, S. (2010). Who is responsible for what? Clarity of responsibilities in multilevel states: The case of Spain. European Journal of Political Research, 50(1), 80–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Listhaug, O. (1995). The dynamics of trust in politicians. In H.-D. Klingemann & D. Fuchs (Eds.), Citizens and the state. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupia, A. (2002). Who can persuade whom? Implications from the nexus of psychology and rational choice theory. In J. H. Kuklinski (Ed.), Thinking about political psychology (pp. 51–88). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lupia, A., & McCubbins, M. D. (1998). The democratic dilemma: Can citizens learn what they need to know?. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maestas, C. D., Atkeson, L. R., Croom, T., & Bryant, L. A. (2008). Shifting the blame: Federalism, media, and public assignment of blame following Hurricane Katrina. Publius, 38, 609–632.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malhotra, N., & Kuo, A. G. (2008). Attributing blame: The public’s response to Hurricane Katrina. Journal of Politics, 70, 120–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, M., & Tilley, J. R. (2010). The attribution of credit and blame to governments and its impact on vote choice. British Journal of Political Science, 40(1), 115–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, R. (2002). Experimental methods in political science. Annual Review of Political Science, 5, 31–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McLaren, L. (2006). Identity, interests and attitudes to European integration. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. T., & Ross, M. (1975). Self-serving biases in the attribution of causality: Fact or fiction? Psychological Bulletin, 82, 213–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mintz, A., Redd, S. B., & Vedlitz, A. (2006). Can we generalize from student experiments to the real world in political science, military affairs, and international relations? Journal of Conflict Resolution, 50, 757–776.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morton, R. B., & Williams, K. C. (2010). From nature to the lab: Experimental political science and the study of causality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nadeau, R., Niemi, R. G., & Yoshinak, A. (2002). A cross-national analysis of economic voting: Taking account of the political context across time and nations. Electoral Studies, 21, 403–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niemi, R.G., Stanley, H.W. &Vogel, R.J. (1995). State economies and state Taxes: Do voters hold governors accountable?. American Journal of Political Science 39(4): 936–957.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., & Wegener, D. T. (1998). Attitude change: Multiple roles for persuasion variables. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Friske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology, vol 2 I (4th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell, G. B., & Whitten, G. D. (1993). A cross-national analysis of economic voting: taking account of the political context. American Journal of Political Science, 37, 391–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rudolph, T. J. (2003a). Who’s responsible for the economy? The formation and consequences of responsibility attributions. American Journal of Political Science, 47, 698–713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rudolph, T. J. (2003b). Institutional context and the assignment of political responsibility. Journal of Politics, 65, 190–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudolph, T. J. (2006) Triangulating political responsibility: The motivated formation of responsibility judgments. Political Psychology, 27(1), 99–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sniderman, P. M. (2000). Taking sides: A fixed choice theory of political reasoning. In A. Lupia, M. D. McCubbins, & S. L. Popkin (Eds.), Elements of reason: Cognition, choice and the bounds of rationality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strøm, K. (1990). A behavioral theory of competitive political parties. American Journal of Political Science, 34(2), 565–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Fiske, S. T. (1975). Point-of-view and perceptions of causality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 439–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Fiske, S. T. (1978). Salience, attention, and attributions: Top of the head phenomena. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 11. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tilley, J. R., & Hobolt, S. B. (2011). Is the government to blame? An experimental test of how partisanship shapes perceptions of performance and responsibility. The Journal of Politics, 73(2), 316–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watts, M. D., Domke, D., Shah, D. V., & Fan, D. P. (1999). Elite cues and media bias in presidential campaigns: Explaining public perceptions of a liberal press. Communication Research, 26, 144–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitten, G. D., & Palmer, H. D. (1999). Cross-national analyses of economic voting. Electoral Studies, 18, 49–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Financial support for this project was gratefully received from the British Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC Grant No. RES-062- 23-1522) and Jesus College Major Research Grants Fund. The authors would also like to thank the staff and participants at the Centre for Experimental Social Science at Nuffield College, Oxford, and Robert Johns and the three anonymous reviewers for insightful comments on the paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sara B. Hobolt.

Appendix: News Articles Given to Participants

Appendix: News Articles Given to Participants

(Bold type indicates positive/negative frame, italic type indicates EU/government source)

Thank you for answering those questions. Next we would like you to read an article about the economic situation in Britain at the moment. Please read the report carefully.

1 June 2009

Economic downturn “less severe/more severe than feared” says the government/says the EU

The economic downturn in Britain will be less/more deep than previously forecast with unemployment staying below 8/exceeding 12 per cent, according to new government figures/new figures from the EU.

The Treasury/The European Commission has stated that Britain’s economy will shrink by less than 2/more than 4 per cent this year, a contraction that is half/double the level it estimated only 4 months ago. The government/EU said in January that it expected the British economy to shrink 3.9/2.1 per cent this year but its outlook is now much more optimistic/pessimistic. It also now expects a small increase/decrease in 2010 compared to its January forecast of a 0.4 per cent contraction/0.4 per cent growth.

Unemployment is expected to remain below 8/exceed 12 per cent by the end of next year. The government/EU has forecast that less than ½/more than 2 million jobs will be lost across Britain by the end of 2010, the government says/government admits /EU says that this means the British economy will have been one of the least/most affected in Europe by the global downturn.

Alistair Darling, Chancellor of the Exchequer,/Joaquin Almunia, European Commissioner for Economic and Monetary Affairs, said: “The British economy is in the midst of its deepest and most widespread recession in the post-war era, but/and the measures taken in these exceptional circumstances are expected to/may not put a floor under the fall in economic activity this year and enable a recovery next year.”

Mr Darling/Mr Almunia told a press conference that many encouraging/discouraging economic signals have recently emerged, pointing to both the turbulence on financial markets and new business confidence figures. “We may no longer/still be in free-fall”, he said.

The government/the EU forecasts say “the likelihood that economic activity in 2010 will be stronger/weaker than previously envisaged” will contain any/mean a further deterioration in the public finances. The government previously announced an extra £700 billion in borrowing over the next 5 years to plug the gap in the public finances as tax receipts plunge and spending on unemployment benefits and stimulus measures rises. But the higher/lower than forecast tax take and less sharp/sharper than forecast decline in economic activity should slow/speed up the rise in the government’s debts as a share of output, the government/EU report said.

Next we would like you to read an article about health care in Britain at the moment. Please read the report carefully.

1 June 2009

UK life expectancy “among the worst/best in Europe” says/admits government/says EU as waiting lists also rise/fall

The average life expectancy of men and women in Britain ranks alongside some of the poorest/richest countries in Europe, according to new government figures/figures from the EU.

Britain comes 21st/4th out of 25 European countries in a new table of life expectancy published by the Department for Health/European Commission today. The figures place women’s longevity in Britain below/above that of all major EU countries including France, Germany, Italy and Spain.

The figures were described as “unacceptable”/“acceptable” by opposition politicians, who pointed out that they don’t factor in the wide variations in life expectancy within Britain. Doctors warn that people living in some parts of Glasgow can expect to live up to three decades less than those living in the Home Counties.

Although men’s life expectancies were generally shorter than women’s, they were lower/higher compared to other countries the Department of Health/European Commission reported. Norway and Denmark were named as the countries where men can expect to live the longest after the age of 50, and Denmark came top of the league table for women. At 79/86 years and 8 months the average life expectancy for women in Britain is considerably shorter/longer than the European average of 83 years and 6 months. Men fare slightly worse overall, with their average life span of 77 years and 6 months, and/although this was below/above the EU average by nearly 3 years.

This report comes less than a week after other government/EU figures showed that NHS waiting lists had risen/fallen over the last year. These government/EU statistics show the number of patients waiting to be admitted to NHS hospitals in England has risen/fallen 19.8% in the last year. Alan Johnson, the Secretary of State for Health,/Androulla Vassiliou, the EU Health Commissioner, said: “Waiting lists in the UK have risen/fallen significantly this year, and our prediction is for them to continue to rise/fall over the coming year”.

He/She said: “Waiting for an appointment can be worrying for patients and reducing waiting times should be a priority. Patients who need urgent treatment need to get it and those who don’t need to be informed and supported to avoid unnecessary anxiety.”

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hobolt, S.B., Tilley, J. & Wittrock, J. Listening to the Government: How Information Shapes Responsibility Attributions. Polit Behav 35, 153–174 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-011-9183-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-011-9183-8

Keywords

Navigation