Skip to main content
Log in

Considering the role of cognitive control in expert performance

  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Dreyfus and Dreyfus’ (1986) influential phenomenological analysis of skill acquisition proposes that expert performance is guided by non-cognitive responses which are fast, effortless and apparently intuitive in nature. Although this model has been criticised (e.g., by Breivik Journal of Philosophy of Sport, 34, 116–134 2007, Journal of the Philosophy of Sport, 40, 85–106 2013; Eriksen 2010; Montero Inquiry: An interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy, 53, 105–122 2010; Montero and Evans 2011) for over-emphasising the role that intuition plays in facilitating skilled performance, it does recognise that on occasions (e.g., when performance goes awry for some reason) a form of ‘detached deliberative rationality’ may be used by experts to improve their performance. However, Dreyfus and Dreyfus (1986) see no role for calculative problem solving or deliberation (i.e., drawing on rules or mental representations) when performance is going well. In the current paper, we draw on empirical evidence, insights from athletes, and phenomenological description to argue that ‘continuous improvement’ (i.e., the phenomenon whereby certain skilled performers appear to be capable of increasing their proficiency even though they are already experts; Toner and Moran 2014) among experts is mediated by cognitive (or executive) control in three distinct sporting situations (i.e., in training, during pre-performance routines, and while engaged in on-line skill execution). We conclude by arguing that Sutton et al. Journal of the British Society for Phenomenology, 42, 78–103 (2011) ‘applying intelligence to the reflexes’ (AIR) approach may help to elucidate the process by which expert performers achieve continuous improvement through analytical/mindful behaviour during training and competition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abernethy, B., Burgess-Limerick, R., & Parks, S. (1994). Contrasting approaches to the study of motor expertise. Quest, 46, 186–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, J., Cote, J., & Deakin, J. (2005). Expertise in ultra-endurance triathletes early sport involvement, training structure, and the theory of deliberate practice. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 17, 64–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernier, M., Codron, R., Thienot, E., & Fournier, J. F. (2011). The attentional focus of expert golfers in training and competition: a naturalistic investigation. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 23, 326–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bissell, D. (2013). Habit displaced: the disruption of skillful performance. Geographical Research, 51, 120–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breivik, G. (2007). Skillful coping in everyday life and in sport: a critical examination of the views of Heidegger and Dreyfus. Journal of the Philosophy of Sport, 34, 116–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breivik, G. (2013). Zombie-like or superconscious? A phenomenological and conceptual analysis of consciousness in elite sport. Journal of the Philosophy of Sport, 40, 85–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carson, H. J., Collins, D., & MacNamara, A. (2013). Systems for technical refinement in experienced performers: the case from expert-level golf. International Journal of Golf Science, 2, 65–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carson, H. J., Collins, D., & Jones, B. (2014). A case study of technical change and rehabilitation: intervention design and interdisciplinary team interaction. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 45, 57–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carter, I. (2012). Michael Bannon: The man who mentors McIlroy. Retrieved from http://www.bbc.co.uk/sport/0/golf/20488972

  • Christensen, W., Sutton, J., & McIlwain, D. (2014). Cognitive control in skilled action. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, in press.

  • Collins, D., Morriss, C., & Trower, J. (1999). Getting it back: a case study of skill recovery in an elite athlete. The Sport Psychologist, 13, 288–298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cotterill, S. T., Sanders, R., & Collins, D. (2010). Developing effective pre-performance routines in golf: Why don’t we ask the golfer? Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 22, 51–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deakin, J., & Cobley, S. (2003). A search for deliberate practice: An examination of the practice environments in figure skating and volleyball. In J. Starkes & K. A. Ericsson (Eds.), Expert performance in sports: Advances in research on sport expertise (pp. 115–136). Champaign, USA: Human Kinetics.

  • Dreyfus, H. (1991). Being-in-the-world: A commentary on Heidegger’s being and time. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, H. (1997). Intuitive, deliberative, and calculative models of expert performance. In C. Zsambok & G. Klein (Eds.), Naturalistic decision making (pp. 17–28). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, H. (2002). Intelligence without representation: Merleau Ponty’s critique of representation. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 1, 367–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, H. (2007). The return of the myth of the mental. Inquiry, 50, 352–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, H., & Dreyfus, S. (1986). Mind over machine. New York: The Free press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, H., & Dreyfus, S. (2004). The ethical implications of the five-stage skill-acquisition model. Bulletin of Science, Technology & Society, 24, 251–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, D. W., Ward, P., & Woodman, T. (2009). Competition specific preparation and expert performance. Psychology of Sport & Exercise, 10, 96–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eden, S. (2013). Stroke of madness: How has Tiger Woods managed to overhaul his swing three times? Retrieved from http://espn.go.com/golf/story//id/8865487/tiger-woods-reinvents-golf-swing-third-time-career-espn-magazine

  • Ericsson, K. A. (2006). The influence of experience and deliberate practice on the development of superior expert performance. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 685–706). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Ward, P. (2007). Capturing the naturally occurring superior performance of experts in the laboratory: toward a science of expert and exceptional performance. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 346–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, J. W. (2010). Mindless coping in competitive sport: some implications and consequences. Sport, Ethics, and Philosophy, 4, 66–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitts, P. M., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human performance. California: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., & Marcel, A. J. (1999). The self in contextualized action. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, 4–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geeves, A., McIlwain, D. J. F., Sutton, J., & Christensen, W. (2014). To think or not to think: the apparent paradox of expert skill in music performance. Educational Philosophy and Theory, 46, 674–691.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gucciardi, D. F., & Dimmock, J. A. (2008). Choking under pressure in sensorimotor skills: conscious processing or depleted attentional resources? Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 9, 45–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hanin, Y., & Hanina, M. (2009). Optimization of performance in Top-level athletes: an action-focused coping approach. International Journal of Sports Science and Coaching, 4, 47–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hanin, Y., Korsus, T., Jouste, P., & Baxter, P. (2002). Rapid technique correction using old way/new way: Two case studies with Olympic athletes. The Sport Psychologist, 16, 79–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanin, Y., Malvela, M., & Hanina, M. (2004). Rapid correction of start technique in an Olympic-level swimmer: a case study using old way/new way. Journal of Swimming Research, 16, 11–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helsen, W. F., Hodges, N. J., & Starkes, J. L. (1998). Team sports and the theory of deliberate practice. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 20, 260–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodgetts, R. (2013). Todd Hamilton: The day in the life of an Open champion. Retrieved from http://www.bbc.co.uk/sport/0/golf/23331505

  • Jackson, S. A., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). Flow in sports: The keys to optimal experiences and performances. Champaign: Human Kinetics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, S. (2007). The Use of swing keys by elite tournament professional golfers. Annual Review of Golf Coaching, 1, 199–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kingston, K. M., & Hardy, L. (1997). Effects of different types of goals on processes that support performance. The Sport Psychologist, 11, 277–293.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacPherson, A., Collins, D., & Morriss, C. (2008). Is what you think what you get? Optimizing mental focus for technical performance. The Sport Psychologist, 22, 288–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marchant, D. C. (2008). Attentional focus and golf performance. Sport and Exercise Psychology Review, 4, 16–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masters, K. S., & Ogles, B. M. (1998). The relations of cognitive strategies with injuries, motivation, and performance among marathon runners: results from two studies. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 10, 281–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of perception (C. Smith, Trans). London and New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moe, V. F. (2005). A philosophical critique of classic cognitivism in sport: from information processing to bodily background knowledge. Journal of the Philosophy of Sport, 32, 155–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montero, B. (2010). Does bodily awareness interfere with highly skilled movement? Inquiry: An interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy, 53, 105–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montero, B. G., & Evans, C. (2011). Intuitions without concepts lose the game: mindedness in the Art of chess. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 10, 175–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moors, A. (2013). Automaticity. In D. L. Reisberg (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive psychology (pp. 163–175). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J. B., Ezekiel, F., & Wilk, H. A. (2011). Cognitive control: easy to identify but hard to define. Topics in Cognitive Science, 3, 212–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, R., & Hardy, L. (2010). Conscious processing and the process goal paradox. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 32, 275–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, A. R., & Polman, R. C. J. (2008). Think aloud: acute stress and coping strategies during golf performances. Anxiety, Stress & Coping, 21, 283–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nyberg, G. (2014). Developing a ‘somatic velocimeter’—the practical knowledge of freeskiers. Qualitative Research in Sport, Exercise and Health, in press.

  • Ravn, S., & Christensen, M. K. (2014). Listening to the body? How phenomenological insights can be used to explore a golfer’s experiences of the physicality of her body. Qualitative Research in Sport, Exercise and Health, 6, 462–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Serfaty, D., MacMillan, J., Entin, E. E., & Entin, E. B. (1997). The decision-making expertise of battle commanders. In C. E. Zsambok & G. A. Klein (Eds.), Naturalistic decision making (pp. 233–246). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shipnuck, A. (2014). With his dominant and historic U.S Open victory, Martin Kaymer solidified himself as the game’s best player. Retrieved from http://www.golf.com/tour-and-news/2014-us-open-martin-kaymer-wins-his-second-major-and-best-player-world

  • Shusterman, R. (2008). Body consciousness: A philosophy of mindfulness and somaesthetics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Shusterman, R. (2009). Body consciousness and performance: somaesthetics east and west. The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism, 67, 133–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shusterman, R. (2011). Soma, self, and society: somaesthetics as pragmatist meliorism. Metaphilosophy, 42, 314–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shusterman, R. (2012). Thinking through the body: Essays in Somaesthetics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, A. M., Adler, C. H., Crews, D., Wharen, R. E., Laskowski, E. E., Barnes, K., Bell, C. V., Pelz, D., Brennan, R. D., Smith, J., Sorenson, M. C., & Kaufman, K. R. (2003). The “yips” in golf: a continuum between a focal dystonia and choking. Sports Medicine, 33, 13–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanley, J., & Krakauer, J. W. (2013). Motor skill depends on knowledge of facts. Frontiers in Human Neurosciences, 7, 503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suss, J., & Ward, P. (2010). Skill-based differences in the cognitive mechanisms underlying failure under stress. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, 54, 1062–1066.

  • Sutton, J. (2007). Batting, habit and memory: the embodied mind and the nature of skill. Sport in Society, 10, 763–786.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, J., Mcilwain, D., Christensen, W., & Geeves, A. (2011). Applying intelligence to the reflexes: embodied skills and habits between Dreyfus and Descartes. Journal of the British Society for Phenomenology, 42, 78–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toner, J. (2014). Knowledge of facts mediate ‘continuous improvement’ in elite sport: a comment on Stanley and Krakauer (2013). Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toner, J., & Moran, A. (2014). In praise of conscious awareness: a new framework for the investigation of ‘continuous improvement’ in athletes. Frontiers in Psychology: Cognition, 5, 769.

  • Toner, J., & Moran, A. (2015). Enhancing performance proficiency at the expert level: considering the role of ‘somaesthetic awareness’. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 16, 110–117.

  • Verbruggen, F., McLaren, I. P. L., & Chambers, C. D. (2014). Banishing the control homunculi in studies of action control and behaviour change. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 497–524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John Toner.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Toner, J., Montero, B.G. & Moran, A. Considering the role of cognitive control in expert performance. Phenom Cogn Sci 14, 1127–1144 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-014-9407-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-014-9407-6

Keywords

Navigation