Skip to main content
Log in

The joy of pain and the pain of joy: In-group identification predicts schadenfreude and gluckschmerz following rival groups’ fortunes

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Four studies examined how in-group identification in the domain of sports is associated with schadenfreude in reaction to another group’s suffering or gluckschmerz in reaction to another group’s good fortune. Schadenfreude increased as a function of in-group identification when the outgroup was a rival team rather than a non-rival team in Study 1. Study 2 showed that those who experience schadenfreude at learning of an outgroup player’s injury will also tend to feel gluckschmerz when they learn of the player’s recovery. Studies 3 and 4 replicated and extended these findings for both schadenfreude and gluckschmerz, and showed that neither the degree of severity of an injury nor the level of physical pain associated with the injury moderated the link between identification and both schadenfreude and gluckschmerz. Mediation analyses indicated that perceived in-group gain or loss, deservedness, and dislike were prime mediators of links between in-group identification and both emotions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Outliers on schadenfreude and negative affect within the Oregon and Duke loss conditions were removed. Two participants scoring more than 1.5 times the interquartile range on schadenfreude in the Oregon condition were removed, while three participants more than 1.5 times the interquartile range on negative feelings over the Duke loss were removed. Including these observations eliminated the main effect of in-group identification on schadenfreude but the pattern of regression results was otherwise similar to those found for the data without outliers.

  2. Four outliers were removed from analyses on the basis of exceeding conventional cutoffs for at least two of the following measures in regressions of schadenfreude against in-group identification: leverage [cutoff = 3((k + 1)/n)], ESR (cutoff = ±3), and DFBetas (cutoff = ±1).

  3. Participants in this condition (n = 164) either read that the player would miss an entire season (n = 111) or would very likely never play basketball competitively again (n = 53). Results were very similar across these variants and thus we combined them to form a single severe injury condition.

  4. Neither of the two-way interaction terms nor the three-way interaction was statistically significant, ps > .30, so the interaction terms were dropped from the presence of schadenfreude model. The final model Cox–Snell R 2 was .15, p < .001.

  5. Neither of the two-way interaction terms nor the three-way interaction was statistically significant, ps > .50, so the interaction terms were dropped from the intensity of schadenfreude model. The final model R 2 was .16, p < .001.

  6. Dislike was a significant mediator in single-mediator models for both the presence of gluckschmerz, OR 1.18, 95 % CI (1.07–1.32), and the log-intensity of gluckschmerz when experienced, b = 0.04, SE = 0.01, 95 % CI (0.02–0.07).

  7. A modest-but-significant zero-order correlation between in-group identification and happiness over the pain was present, r = .12, p = .048, τ = .10, p = .031, but controlling for gender and general fandom reduced this effect to nonsignificance, r = .07, p = .22, τ = .06, p = .13.

  8. The strength of the associations between in-group identification and schadenfreude and gluckschmerz was descriptively higher among the subset of participants who read about a career-ending injury than for the other participants in Study 4; controlling for gender and general fandom, r schadenfreude = .35 versus .28, r gluckschmerz = .38 versus .34.

  9. In another study, we replicated the basic finding for in-group identification using a player for a different rival team, the University of North Carolina (UNC). (Kentucky and UNC are ranked 1 and 3 in total number of all time victories.) Ninety participants read an article indicating that Kendall Marshall of UNC’s men’s basketball team had successful surgery for his wrist, but would most likely be out for the rest of the NCAA tournament. Controlling for sport fandom, in-group identification was significantly related to schadenfreude in reaction to reading about Marshall’s wrist injury, b = 0.24, SE = 0.10, t = 2.35, p = .021; in single mediation analyses, perceived in-group gain, perceptions of deservedness, and dislike of UNC mediated this effect. Gain and deservedness were significant mediators in the multiple mediation analysis with all three potential mediators.

  10. In light of the close logical and empirical links between schadenfreude and gluckschmerz, one might question whether gluckschmerz occurs when one has not previously learned of a misfortune suffered by the other. A “satisfying” misfortune may set up a necessary contrast through a reversal of fortunes. To address this possibility, we conducted a follow-up to Study 3 in which we found that gluckschmerz can occur in the absence of a counter-expectancy (in this case, the belief that an outgroup player would remain injured). Participants read only the article about Rivers’ recovery. Controlling for general sport fandom, in-group identification was positively related to gluckschmerz (M = 1.49, SD = 1.62), b = 0.36, SE = 0.12, t = 3.02, p = .004. Gluckschmerz apparently does not require previous schadenfreude, nor is the previous experience of implied in-group gain due to an outgroup misfortune necessary to feel a sense of in-group loss when the outgroup benefits from a member’s renewed ability to actively contribute.

References

  • Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Ze’ev, A. (1992). Cognitive development: Two paradigms. In H. Geissler, S. W. Link, & J. T. Townsend (Eds.), Cognition, information processing, and psychophysics: Basic issues (pp. 67–90). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruneau, E., Dufour, N., & Saxe, R. (2013). How we know it hurts: Item analysis of written narratives reveals distinct neural responses to others’ physical pain and emotional suffering. PLoS ONE, 8(4), e63085. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0063085.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chand, S., & Aftab, N. (2012). On finite sample distribution of quasi test statistic based on heteroskedastic consistent covariance matrix estimators. Middle-East Journal of Scientific Research, 12(8), 1157–1164. doi:10.5829/idosi.mejsr.2012.12.8.1852.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini, R. B., Borden, R. J., Thome, A., Walker, M. R., Freeman, S., & Sloan, L. R. (1976). Basking in reflected glory: Three (football) field studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34(3), 366–375. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.34.3.366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cikara, M., Botvinick, M. M., & Fiske, S. T. (2011a). Us versus them: Social identity shapes neural responses to intergroup competition and harm. Psychological Science, 22(3), 306–313. doi:10.1177/0956797610397667.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cikara, M., Bruneau, E. G., & Saxe, R. R. (2011b). Us and them: Intergroup failures of empathy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(3), 149–153. doi:10.1177/0963721411408713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cikara, M., & Fiske, S. T. (2012). Stereotypes and schadenfreude: Affective and physiological markers of pleasure at outgroups’ misfortunes. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(1), 63–71. doi:10.1177/1948550611409245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Combs, D. Y., Powell, C. J., Schurtz, D., & Smith, R. H. (2009). Politics, schadenfreude, and ingroup identification: The sometimes happy thing about a poor economy and death. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(4), 635–646. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.02.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cribari-Neto, F., & da Silva, W. B. (2011). A new heteroskedasticity-consistent covariance matrix estimator for the linear regression model. AStA Advances in Statistical Analysis, 95(2), 129–146. doi:10.1007/s10182-010-0141-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cuddy, A. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2007). The BIAS map: Behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(4), 631–648. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.4.631.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Efron, B. (1987). Better bootstrap confidence-intervals. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 82(397), 171–185. doi:10.2307/2289144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feather, N. T. (2006). Deservingness and emotions: Applying the structural model of deservingness to the analysis of affective reactions to outcomes. European Review of Social Psychology, 17(1), 38–73. doi:10.1080/10463280600662321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feather, N. T., & Sherman, R. (2002). Envy, resentment, schadenfreude, and sympathy: Reactions to deserved and undeserved achievement and subsequent failure. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(7), 953–961. doi:10.1177/014616720202800708.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Featherston, A. (2012). A look at the Duke-Kentucky series. GoDuke.com. Retrieved from http://www.goduke.com/ViewArticle.dbml?ATCLID=205731187.

  • Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 878–902. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.878.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gaertner, L., & Insko, C. A. (2000). Intergroup discrimination in the minimal group paradigm: Categorization, reciprocation, or fear? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(1), 77–94. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.79.1.77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gay, Peter. (1998). My German question: Growing up in Nazi Berlin. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hareli, S., & Weiner, B. (2002). Dislike and envy as antecedents of pleasure at another’s misfortune. Motivation and Emotion, 26(4), 257–277. doi:10.1023/A:1022818803399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). An introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hornsey, M. J. (2008). Social identity theory and self-categorization theory: A historical review. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(1), 204–222. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00066.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krach, S., Cohrs, J. C., de Echeverría Loebell, N. C., Kircher, T., Sommer, J., et al. (2011). Your flaws are my pain: Linking empathy to vicarious embarrassment. PLoS ONE, 6(4), e18675. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0018675.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leach, C. W., & Spears, R. (2008). “A vengefulness of the impotent”: The pain of in-group inferiority and schadenfreude toward successful out-groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(6), 1383–1396. doi:10.1037/a0012629.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leach, C. W., & Spears, R. (2009). Dejection at in-group defeat and schadenfreude toward second- and third-party out-groups. Emotion, 9(5), 659–665. doi:10.1037/a0016815.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leach, C. W., Spears, R., Branscombe, N. R., & Doosje, B. (2003). Malicious pleasure: Schadenfreude at the suffering of another group. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(5), 932–943. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.932.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luhtanen, R., & Crocker, J. (1992). A collective self-esteem scale: Self-evaluation of one’s social identity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18(3), 302–318. doi:10.1177/0146167292183006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mackie, D. M., Devos, T., & Smith, E. R. (2000). Intergroup emotions: Explaining offensive action tendencies in an intergroup context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(4), 602–616. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.602.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oakes, P. J., & Turner, J. C. (1980). Social categorization and inter-group behavior—does minimal inter-group discrimination make social identity more positive? European Journal of Social Psychology, 10(3), 295–301. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420100307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olsen, M. K., & Schafer, J. L. (2001). A two-part random-effects model for semicontinuous longitudinal data. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 96(454), 730–745. doi:10.1198/016214501753168389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ouwerkerk, J. W., & van Dijk, W. W. (2014). Intergroup rivalry and schadenfreude. In W. W. van Dijk, & J. W. Ouwerkerk (Eds.), Schadenfreude: Understanding pleasure at the misfortune of others (pp. 186–199). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

  • Portmann, J. (2000). When bad things happen to other people. New York: Routledge.

  • Smith, R. H. (2013). The joy of pain: Schadenfreude and the dark side of human nature. New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Smith, R. H., Eyre, H. L., Powell, C. J., & Kim, S. (2006). Relativistic origins of emotional reactions to events happening to others and to ourselves. British Journal of Social Psychology, 45(2), 357–371. doi:10.1348/014466605X40987.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, R. H., & Kim, S. (2007). Comprehending envy. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 46–64. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, R. H., Powell, C. J., Combs, D. Y., & Schurtz, D. (2009). Exploring the when and why of Schadenfreude. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3(4), 530–546. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00181.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, R. H., Turner, T. J., Garonzik, R., Leach, C. W., Urch-Druskat, V., & Weston, C. M. (1996). Envy and Schadenfreude. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(2), 158–168. doi:10.1177/0146167296222005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dijk, W. W., Goslinga, S., & Ouwerkerk, J. W. (2008). The impact of responsibility for a misfortune on schadenfreude and sympathy: Further evidence. Journal of Social Psychology, 148(5), 631–636. doi:10.3200/SOCP.148.5.631-636.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dijk, W. W., & Ouwerkerk, J. W. (2014). Schadenfreude: Theory and research. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

  • Van Dijk, W. W., Ouwerkerk, J. W., Goslinga, S., & Nieweg, M. (2005). Deservingness and schadenfreude. Cognition and Emotion, 19(6), 933–939. doi:10.1080/02699930541000066.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dijk, W. W., Ouwerkerk, J. W., Goslinga, S., Nieweg, M., & Gallucci, M. (2006). When people fall from grace: Reconsidering the role of envy in schadenfreude. Emotion, 6(1), 156–160. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.6.1.156.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dijk, W. W., Ouwerkerk, J. W., Wesseling, Y. M., & Van Koningsbruggen, G. M. (2011). Towards understanding pleasure at the misfortunes of others: The impact of self-evaluation threat on schadenfreude. Cognition and Emotion, 25(2), 360–368. doi:10.1080/02699931.2010.487365.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wann, D. L., & Branscombe, N. R. (1993). Sports fans: Measuring degree of identification with their team. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 24(1), 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wann, D. L., Fahl, C. L., Erdmann, J. B., & Littleton, J. D. (1999). Relationship between identification with the role of sport fan and trait aggression. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 88(3, Pt. 2), 1296–1298. doi:10.2466/PMS.88.3.1296-1298.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wann, D. L., Haynes, G., McLean, B., & Pullen, P. (2003). Sport team identification and willingness to consider anonymous acts of hostile aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 29(5), 406–413. doi:10.1002/ab.10046.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wann, D. L., Waddill, P. J., & Dunham, M. D. (2004). Using sex and gender role orientation to predict level of sport fandom. Journal of Sport Behavior, 27(4), 367–377.

  • Wills, T. A. (1981). Downward social comparison principles in social psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 90(1), 245–271. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.90.2.245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yzerbyt, V. Y., Muller, D., & Judd, C. M. (2004). Adjusting researchers’ approach to adjustment: On the use of covariates when testing interactions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40(3), 424–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhong, C. B., Phillips, K. W., Leonardelli, G. J., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Negational categorization and intergroup behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(6), 793–806. doi:10.1177/0146167208315457.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank John Martin, Crystal Neace, Alex Bianchi, and Chris Thompson for their assistance in data collection and entry.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Charles E. Hoogland.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hoogland, C.E., Ryan Schurtz, D., Cooper, C.M. et al. The joy of pain and the pain of joy: In-group identification predicts schadenfreude and gluckschmerz following rival groups’ fortunes. Motiv Emot 39, 260–281 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-014-9447-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-014-9447-9

Keywords

Navigation