Skip to main content
Log in

Contrasting the effects of suboptimally versus optimally presented affect primes on effort-related cardiac response

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Participants worked on an easy versus difficult arithmetic task with integrated happiness versus sadness primes, presented either suboptimally (briefly and masked) or optimally (long and visible). As predicted by the IAPE model (Gendolla in International Journal of Psychophysiology 86:123–135, 2012. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.05.003), the affect primes moderated the task difficulty effect on mental effort in the suboptimal-prime condition: cardiac pre-ejection period response was stronger in the happiness/difficult than in the sadness/difficult condition and tended to be stronger in the sadness-easy than in the happiness-easy condition. These effects were reversed in the optimal-prime-presentation condition, suggesting behavior correction due to controlled prime processing. Moreover, neither suboptimally nor optimally presented affect primes had prime-congruent effects on conscious mood assessed via self-report. The results demonstrate differential effects of implicitly versus explicitly processed affect cues on mental effort and suggest that they can do so without inducing emotional feelings.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We use the term suboptimal rather than subliminal, because the latter refers to stimulus presentations below individually determined thresholds of conscious perception. In our experiments, low contrast affective stimuli are briefly presented (27 ms) and backward masked resulting in suboptimal presentation in order to prevent controlled processing of the primes’ content.

  2. Due to technical measurement problems, there were missing data for some participants. Therefore, the sample sizes slightly varied across the analysis of the dependent variables: N = 134 for PEP, N = 133 for HR, and N = 130 for SBP and DBP.

References

  • Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54, 462–479. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Belani, K. G., Buckley, J. J., & Poliac, M. O. (1999). Accuracy of radial artery blood pressure determination with the Vasotrac™. Canadian Journal of Anesthesia/Journal Canadien d’Anesthésie, 46, 488–496. doi:10.1007/BF03012951.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berntson, G. G., Cacioppo, J. T., & Quigley, K. S. (1993). Cardiac psychophysiology and autonomic space in humans: Empirical perspectives and conceptual implications. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 296–322. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.114.2.296.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berntson, G. G., Lozano, D. L., Chen, Y.-J., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). Where to Q in PEP. Psychophysiology, 41, 333–337. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00156.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bijleveld, E., Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2010). Unconscious reward cues increase invested effort, but do not change speed–accuracy tradeoffs. Cognition, 115, 330–335. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.12.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bijleveld, E., Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2012). Human reward pursuit: From rudimentary to higher-level functions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 194–199. doi:10.1177/096372141243846.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brehm, J. W. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brehm, J. W., & Self, E. A. (1989). The intensity of motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 109–131. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.40.020189.000545.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clore, G. L., Storbeck, J., Robinson, M. D., & Centerbar, D. B. (2005). Seven sins in the study of unconscious affect. In L. F. Barrett, P. M. Niedenthal, & P. Winkielman (Eds.), Emotion and consciousness (pp. 384–408). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Spruyt, A., & Moors, A. (2009). Implicit measures: A normative analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 347–368. doi:10.1037/a0014211.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., & Naccache, L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition, 79, 1–37. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00123-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeMarree, K. G., Loersch, C., Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., Payne, B. K., & Rucker, D. D. (2012). From primed construct to motivated behavior: Validation processes in goal pursuit. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 1659–1670. doi:10.1177/0146167212458328.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis, A., & Aarts, H. (2010). Goals, attention, and consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 461–490. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freydefont, L., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2012). Incentive moderates the impact of implicit anger versus sadness cues on effort-related cardiac response. Biological Psychology, 91, 120–127. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.04.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freydefont, L., Gendolla, G. H. E., & Silvestrini, N. (2012). Beyond valence: The differential effect of masked anger and sadness stimuli on effort-related cardiac response. Psychophysiology, 49, 665–671. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01340.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E. (2012). Implicit affect primes effort: A theory and research on cardiovascular response. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 86, 123–135. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.05.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., & Brinkmann, K. (2005). The role of mood states in self-regulation. European Psychologist, 10, 187–198. doi:10.1027/1016-9040.10.3.187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., Brinkmann, K., & Silvestrini, N. (2012). Gloomy and lazy? On the impact of mood and depressive symptoms on effort-related cardiovascular response. In R. A. Wright & G. H. E. Gendolla (Eds.), How motivation affects cardiovascular response: Mechanisms and applications (pp. 139–155). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., & Krüsken, J. (2002). Informational mood impact on effort-related cardiovascular response: Moods’ diagnostic value counts. Emotion, 2, 251–261. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.2.3.251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., & Richter, M. (2010). Effort mobilization when the self is involved: Some lessons from the cardiovascular system. Review of General Psychology, 14, 212–226. doi:10.1037/a0019742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., & Silvestrini, N. (2010). The implicit “Go”: Masked action cues directly mobilize mental effort. Psychological Science, 21, 1389–1393. doi:10.1177/0956797610384149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., & Silvestrini, N. (2011). Smiles make it easier and so do frowns: Masked affective stimuli influence mental effort. Emotion, 11, 320–328. doi:10.1037/a0022593.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., & Silvestrini, N. (2015). Bounded effort automaticity: A drama in four parts. In G. H. E. Gendolla, M. Tops, & S. Koole (Eds.), Handbook of biobehavioral approaches to self-regulation. New York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-1-4939-1236-0_18

  • Herr, P. M. (1986). Consequences of priming: Judgment and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1106–1115. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelsey, R. M. (2012). Beta-adrenergic cardiovascular reactivity and adaptation to stress: The cardiac pre-ejection period as an index of effort. In R. A. Wright & G. H. E. Gendolla (Eds.), How motivation affects cardiovascular response: Mechanisms and applications (pp. 43–60). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kreibig, S. D., Gendolla, G. H. E., & Scherer, K. R. (2012). Goal relevance and goal conduciveness appraisals lead to differential autonomic reactivity in emotional responding to performance feedback. Biological Psychology, 91, 365–375. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.08.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lasauskaite, R., Gendolla, G. H. E., & Silvestrini, N. (2013). Do sadness-primes make me work harder because they make me sad? Cognition and Emotion, 27, 158–165. doi:10.1080/02699931.2012.689756.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levick, J. R. (2003). An introduction to cardiovascular physiology (4th ed.). London: Arnold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Llabre, M. M., Spitzer, S. B., Saab, P. G., Ironson, G. H., & Schneiderman, N. (1991). The reliability and specificity of delta versus residualized change as measures of cardiovascular reactivity to behavioral challenges. Psychophysiology, 28, 701–711. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1991.tb01017.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loersch, C., & Payne, B. K. (2012). On mental contamination: The role of (mis) attribution in behavior priming. Social Cognition, 30, 241–252. doi:10.1521/soco.2012.30.2.241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lozano, D. L., Norman, G., Knox, D., Wood, B. L., Miller, B. D., Emery, C. F., et al. (2007). Where to B in dZ/dt. Psychophysiology, 44, 113–119. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00468.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lundqvist, D., & Litton, J. E. (1998). The averaged Karolinska directed emotional facesAKDEF. Stockholm, Sweden: CD ROM from Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section, Karolinska Institutet.

  • Matthews, G., Jones, D. M., & Chamberlain, A. G. (1990). Refining the measurement of mood: The UWIST mood adjective checklist. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 17–42. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1990.tb02343.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition, and awareness: Affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 723–739. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.64.5.723.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Obrist, P. A. (1981). Cardiovascular psychophysiology: A perspective. New York: Plenum Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Richter, M. (2010). Blue box 2. Geneva, Switzerland: University of Geneva.

  • Richter, M., Friedrich, A., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2008). Task difficulty effects on cardiac activity. Psychophysiology, 45, 869–875. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00688.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Belief and feeling: Evidence for an accessibility model of emotional self-report. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 934–960. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.128.6.934.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rotteveel, M., de Groot, P., Geutskens, A., & Phaf, R. H. (2001). Stronger suboptimal than optimal affective priming? Emotion, 1, 348–364. doi:10.1037//1528-3542.1.4.348.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 78–88. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scherhag, A., Kaden, J. J., Kentschke, E., Sueselbeck, T., & Borggrefe, M. (2005). Comparison of impedance cardiography and thermodilution-derived measurements of stroke volume and cardiac output at rest and during exercice testing. Cardiovascular Drugs and Therapy, 19, 141–147. doi:10.1007/s10557-005-1048-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sherwood, A., Allen, M. T., Fahrenberg, J., Kelsey, R. M., Lovallo, W. R., & Doornen, L. J. P. (1990). Methodological guidelines for impedance cardiography. Psychophysiology, 27, 1–23. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1990.tb02171.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, P., & Warren, R. (2013). Less is still more: Maintenance of the very brief exposure effect 1 year later. Emotion, 13, 338–344. doi:10.1037/a003083.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, P., & Weinberger, J. (2012). Less is more: The effects of very brief versus clearly visible exposure. Emotion, 12, 394–402. doi:10.1037/a0026806.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Silvestrini, N., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2011a). Do not prime too much: Prime frequency effects of masked affective stimuli on effort-related cardiovascular response. Biological Psychology, 87, 195–199. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.01.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Silvestrini, N., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2011b). Masked affective stimuli moderate task difficulty effects on effort-related cardiovascular response. Psychophysiology, 48, 1157–1164. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01181.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Silvia, P. J. (2012). Mirrors, masks, and motivation: Implicit and explicit self-focused attention influence effort-related cardiovascular reactivity. Biological Psychology, 90, 192–201. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.03.01.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verwijmeren, T., Karremans, J. C., Bernritter, S. F., Stroebe, W., & Wigboldus, D. H. (2013). Warning: You are being primed! The effect of a warning on the impact of subliminal ads. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 1124–1129. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2013.06.01.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winkielman, P., Berridge, K. C., & Wilbarger, J. L. (2005). Emotion, behavior, and conscious experience. In L. F. Barrett, P. M. Niedenthal, & P. Winkielman (Eds.), Emotion and consciousness (pp. 334–362). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winkielman, P., & Schooler, J. W. (2011). Splitting consciousness: Unconscious, conscious, and metaconscious processes in social cognition. European Review of Social Psychology, 22, 1–35. doi:10.1080/10463283.2011.57658.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, R. A. (1996). Brehm’s theory of motivation as a model of effort and cardiovascular response. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action (pp. 424–453). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, R. A. (1998). Ability perception and cardiovascular response to behavioral challenge. In M. Kofta, G. Weary, & G. Sedek (Eds.), Personal control in action: Cognitive and motivational mechanisms (pp. 197–232). New York: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, R. A., & Kirby, L. D. (2001). Effort determination of cardiovascular response: An integrative analysis with applications in social psychology. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 255–307. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(01)80007-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a research grant from the Swiss National Science Foundation awarded to the second author (SNF 100014-131760/1). We thank Audrey Bridy for her help as a hired experimenter.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Guido H. E. Gendolla.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lasauskaite Schüpbach, R., Gendolla, G.H.E. & Silvestrini, N. Contrasting the effects of suboptimally versus optimally presented affect primes on effort-related cardiac response. Motiv Emot 38, 748–758 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-014-9438-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-014-9438-x

Keywords

Navigation