Skip to main content
Log in

Helpful but insufficient: Incremental theory on challenge-confronting tendencies for students who fear being laughed at

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Previous studies have demonstrated that endorsing an incremental theory of intelligence is helpful in motivating students to confront challenges. We used a survey to examine whether this theory could predict greater challenge-confronting tendencies for students with relatively high gelotophobia (i.e., the fear of being laughed at) and explore the possible underlying processes. A total of 264 senior high school students completed the measures of gelotophobia, implicit theories of intelligence, challenge-confronting tendencies, perceived value and feelings of being threatened in confronting challenges, and their self-perceived general ability. The results showed that endorsing an incremental theory predicted greater perceived value in confronting challenges and stronger challenge-confronting tendencies for students with relatively low gelotophobia. However, for those with relatively high gelotophobia, although this theory also predicted greater perceived value in confronting challenges, it was unrelated to challenge-confronting tendencies. Thus, endorsing an incremental theory is helpful but insufficient in motivating high gelotophobia students to exhibit stronger challenge-confronting tendencies.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The analysis of mediated moderation pattern is different from a normal mediation or moderation pattern. According to Muller et al. (2005), the mediation question focuses on the intervening mechanism that produces a treatment effect on an outcome. The moderation question focuses on factors that affect the magnitude of the treatment effect on an outcome. However, the mediated moderation question focuses on the intervening mechanism that produces a moderating effect. That is, how the moderating effect of a factor on the relationship between a treatment and an outcome is produced? Mediated moderation may be produced in either (or both) of two ways (Muller et al., 2005, p. 858). Our data supported the first mediated moderation pattern that the moderating effect of GEL on the relationship between IT and CCT (Model 1) is mediated by the main effect of IT on PV (Model 2) and the moderating effect of GEL on the relationship between PV and CCT (Model 3). In addition, the moderating effect of GEL (Model 1) is also likely to be mediated by the moderating effect of GEL on the relationship between IT and PV (Model 2) and the main effect of PV on CCT (Model 3). This mediated moderation pattern should be supported by the combination of two other significant paths (i.e., the IT × GEL interaction on PV in Model 2 and the main effect of PV on CCT in Model 3). However, the IT × GEL interaction on PV in Model 2 was insignificant in our data.

References

  • Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the classroom: Students’ learning strategies and motivation processes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 260–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aronson, J., Fried, C. B., & Good, C. (2002). Reducing the effects of stereotype threat on African American college students by shaping theories of intelligence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 113–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, I., Smits, D. J. M., Claes, L., Vandereycken, W., & Bijttebier, P. (2009). Psychometric evaluation of the behavioral inhibition/behavioral activation system scales and the sensitivity to punishment and sensitivity to reward questionnaire in a sample of eating disordered patients. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 407–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. Child Development, 78, 246–263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boggiano, A. K., Main, D. S., & Katz, P. A. (1988). Children’s preference for challenge: The role of perceived competence and control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 134–141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carretero-Dios, H., Ruch, W., Agudelo, D., Platt, T., & Proyer, R. T. (2010). Fear of being laughed at and social anxiety: A preliminary psychometric study. Psychological Test and Assessment Modeling, 52, 108–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chan, Y. C., Chen, H. C., & Lavallee, J. P. (2013). The impact of gelotophobia, gelotophilia and katagelasticism on creativity. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 26, 609–628.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cianci, A. M., Klein, H. J., & Seijts, G. H. (2010). The effect of negative feedback on tension and subsequent performance: The main and interactive effects of goal content and conscientiousness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 618–630.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coplan, R. J., Wilson, J., Frohlick, S. L., & Zelenski, J. (2006). A person-oriented analysis of behavioral inhibition and behavioral activation in children. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 917–927.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO PI-R professional manual: Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cury, F., Da Fonseca, D., Zahn, I., & Elliot, A. (2008). Implicit theories and IQ test performance: A sequential mediational analysis. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 783–791.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C. Y., & Hong, Y. Y. (1995). Implicit theories: Elaboration and extension of the model. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 322–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S., & Master, A. (2009). Self-theories and motivation: Student’s beliefs about intelligence. In K. R. Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 123–140). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, J. R., & Lambert, L. S. (2007). Methods for integrating moderation and mediation: A general analytical framework using moderated path analysis. Psychological Methods, 12, 1–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, K., Martin, R. A., & Dozois, D. J. A. (2010). The fear of being laughed at, social anxiety, and memories of being teased during childhood. Psychological Test and Assessment Modeling, 52, 94–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • El-Alayli, A., & Baumgardner, A. (2003). If at first you don’t succeed, what makes you try, try again? Effects of implicit theories and ability feedback in a performance-oriented climate. Self and Identity, 2, 119–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, E. S., & Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach to motivation and achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 397–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Good, C., & Dweck, C. S. (2005). A motivational approach to reasoning, resilience, and responsibility. In R. Stemberg & R. Subotnick (Eds.), Optimizing student success in school reasoning, resilience, and responsibility with the other three R’s (pp. 39–56). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grant, H., & Dweck, C. S. (2003). Clarifying achievement goals and their impact. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 541–553.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A. (1987). The psychology of fear and stress. London: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helmreich, R. L., & Spence, J. T. (1978). Achievement motivation and scientific attainment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4, 222–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hong, Y. Y., Chiu, C. Y., Dweck, C. S., Lin, D. M. S., & Wan, W. (1999). Implicit theories, attributions, and coping: A meaning system approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 588–599.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y. H., Chiu, C. Y., & Zou, Z. (2010). Know thyself: Misperceptions of actual performance undermine achievement motivation, future performance, and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 395–409.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • King, R. B. (2012). How you think about your intelligence influences how adjusted you are: Implicit theories and adjustment outcomes. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 705–709.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kornilova, T. V., Kornilov, S. A., & Chumakova, M. A. (2009). Subjective evaluations of intelligence and academic self-concept predict academic achievement: Evidence from a selective student population. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 596–608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lepper, M. R., Corpus, J. H., & Iyengar, S. S. (2005). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation orientations in the classroom: Age differences and academic correlates. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 184–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, C. H. (2012). Valuable but threatening: The reduced effect of incremental theory on challenge-confronting tendencies for students who fear being laughed at. Learning and Individual Differences, 22, 839–843.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muller, D., Judd, C. M., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2005). When moderation is mediated and mediation is moderated. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 852–863.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Niiya, Y., Crocker, J., & Bartmess, E. N. (2004). From vulnerability to resilience. Psychological Science, 15, 801–805.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nussbaum, A. D., & Dweck, C. S. (2008). Defensiveness versus remediation: Self-theories and modes of self esteem maintenance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 599–612.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A. F., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, W. J. (1991). A manual for the use of the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ). MI: National Center for Research to Improve Postsecondary Teaching and learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Platt, T., & Ruch, W. (2009). The emotions of gelotophobes: Shameful, fearful and joyless? Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 22, 91–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Proyer, R. T., & Ruch, W. (2010). Dispositions towards ridicule and being laughed at: Current research on gelotophobia, gelotophilia, and katagelasticism. Psychological Test and Assessment Modeling, 52, 49–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, S. R., Canada, K. E., & Rarsch, M. K. (2002). Self-handicapping and the five factor model of personality: Mediation between neuroticism and conscientiousness. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 1173–1184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruch, W. (2009). Fearing humor? Gelotophobia: The fear of being laughed at. Introduction and overview. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 22, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruch, W., & Proyer, R. T. (2008). The fear of being laughed at: Individual and group differences in gelotophobia. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 21, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruch, W., & Proyer, R. T. (2009). Extending the study of gelotophobia: On gelotophiles and katagelasticists. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 22, 183–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruch, W., Proyer, R. T., & Popa, D. E. (2008). The fear of being laughed at (gelotophobia) and personality. Anuarul Institutului de Istorie “G. Baritiu” din Cluj-Napoca (Ed.), Series Humanistica (Vol. VI, pp. 53–68). Cluj-Napoca: Romania.

  • Sherman, D. K., & Cohen, J. L. (2006). The psychology of self-defense: Self-affirmation theory. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.). Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 38, pp. 183–242). San Diego. CA: Academic press.

  • Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Titze, M. (2009). Gelotophobia: The fear of being laughed at. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 22, 27–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vohs, K. D., Park, J. K., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2013). Self-affirmation can enable goal disengagement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 14–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 68–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research is mainly supported by the National Science Council, Taiwan, R.O.C. under Grant no. NSC 100-2410-H-364-007-MY2. This research is also partially supported by the “Aim for the Top University Project” of National Taiwan Normal University (NTNU), sponsored by the Ministry of Education, Taiwan, R.O.C.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Cheng-Hong Liu.

Appendix

Appendix

See Table 3.

Table 3 .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Liu, CH., Chiu, FC., Chen, HC. et al. Helpful but insufficient: Incremental theory on challenge-confronting tendencies for students who fear being laughed at. Motiv Emot 38, 367–377 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-013-9386-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-013-9386-x

Keywords

Navigation