Skip to main content
Log in

Emotion and information search in tactical decision-making: Moderator effects of feedback

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Information search during decision-making may be influenced both by selective attention and by the criterion used for ceasing to search. When the items searched are themselves emotive, the affective state of the decision-maker may bias these search processes. If an automatic selective attention bias operates, it may produce mood-congruent effects irrespective of context; e.g., negative affect may focus attention on negative items of information. An alternative possibility, suggested by the mood-as-input model, is that the influence of affect on search may depend on how decision-makers understand their emotions within a given context. The present study tested predictions derived from relevant theories of affective bias, using feedback as a means to generate a context of success or failure. Hundred and sixty participants were required to access positive and negative items of information in choosing between different routes for a search-and-rescue mission. Outcomes of choices were manipulated experimentally, so that in one condition participants received mostly positive feedback, and in a second condition participants received mostly negative feedback. Results showed that the feedback manipulation influenced affect, but there was considerable variation in affective state within each condition. Associations between affect and information search were moderated by feedback condition. For example, positive affect was associated with more frequent sampling of positive information in the negative feedback condition, but the association reversed when feedback was positive. Findings were consistent with the mood-as-input hypothesis, but not with an automatic selective attention bias. Context may influence how the decision-maker interprets their affective state.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. This analysis depends on there being sufficient variability in affective state within each feedback group to differentiate effects of group from effects of affect. Comparison of pre-task and post-task SDs in Table 1 shows that the feedback manipulation did not produce any restriction of range. In addition, although post-task means differed across groups, there was considerable overlap in the distributions of each state across positive and negative feedback conditions.

References

  • Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Amin, Z., Constable, R. T., & Canli, T. (2004). Attentional bias for valenced stimuli as a function of personality in the dot-probe task. Journal of Research in Personality, 38, 15–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H., & Fiedler, K. (2006). Mood and the regulation of information processing and behavior. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Affect in social thinking and behavior (pp. 65–84). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129–148.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H., & Forgas, J. P. (2001). Mood and social memory. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Handbook of affect and social cognition (pp. 95–120). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Browne, G. J., & Pitts, M. G. (2004). Stopping rule use during information search in design problems. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 95, 208–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carnevale, P. J. (2008). Positive affect and decision frame in negotiation. Group Decision and Negotiation, 17, 51–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, F. (2000). On the structure of behavioral self-regulation. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 41–84). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Clore, G. L., & Huntsinger, J. R. (2009). How the object of affect guides its impact. Emotion Review, 1, 39–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corr, P. J. (2009). The reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality. In P. J. Corr & G. Matthews (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of personality psychology (pp. 347–376). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Das, E., & Fennis, B. M. (2008). In the mood to face the facts: When a positive mood promotes systematic processing of self-threatening information. Motivation and Emotion, 32, 221–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M. A. (2009). Understanding the relationship between mood and creativity: A meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 108, 25–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derakshan, N., & Koster, E. H. W. (2010). Processing efficiency in anxiety: Evidence from eye-movements during visual search. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 1180–1185.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (1997). Motivational and attentional components of personality. In G. Matthews (Ed.), Cognitive science perspectives on personality and emotion (pp. 443–473). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Derryberry, D., & Reed, A. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 225–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dhami, M. K., & Harries, C. (2010). Information search in heuristic decision making. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 571–586.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellenbogen, M. A., & Schwartzman, E. (2009). Selective attention and avoidance on a pictorial cueing task during stress in clinically anxious and depressed participants. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 128–138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W. (1992). Anxiety: The cognitive perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., & Derakshan, N. (2011). New perspectives in attentional control theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 955–960.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, S. B., Eysenck, H. J., & Barrett, P. (1985). A revised version of the Psychoticism scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 21–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, P., Kastenmüller, A., Greitemeyer, T., Fischer, J., Frey, D., & Crelley, D. (2011). Threat and selective exposure: The moderating role of threat and decision context on confirmatory information search after decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 51–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fishbach, A., Eyal, T., & Finkelstein, S. R. (2010). How positive and negative feedback motivate goal pursuit. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4, 517–530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P. (1991). Affective influences on partner choice: Role of mood in social decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 708–720.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E., Russo, R., & Dutton, K. (2002). Attentional bias for threat: Evidence for delayed disengagement from emotional faces. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 355–379.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. M., & The ABC Research Group. (1999). Simple heuristics that make us smart. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Revelle, W., & Gotlib, I. H. (2000). Stroop interference following mood induction: Emotionality, mood congruence, concern relevance, and persistence. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24, 491–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H., Krasnoperova, E., Yue, D. N., & Joormann, J. (2004). Attentional biases for negative interpersonal stimuli in clinical depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 13, 127–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, S. G., & Staveland, L. E. (1988). Development of NASA-TLX (task load index): Results of empirical and theoretical research. In P. A. Hancock & N. Meshkati (Eds.), Human mental workload (pp. 139–183). Oxford, UK: North-Holland.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hawksley, J., & Davey, G. C. L. (2010). Mood-as-input and depressive rumination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 134–140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M. (1993). Positive affect and decision-making. In J. M. Haviland & M. Lewis (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 417–436). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2007). Selective attention to emotional faces following recovery from depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 80–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., & Siemer, M. (2004). Memory accessibility, mood regulation and dysphoria: Difficulties in repairing sad mood with happy memories? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 179–188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leung, K., Lee, T. M. C., Yip, P., Li, L. S. W., & Wong, M. M. C. (2009). Selective attention biases of people with depression: Positive and negative priming of depression-related information. Psychiatry Research, 165, 241–251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, E. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 267–286.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R. E., & Baird, M. (2004). Extraversion and emotional reactivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 473–485.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R., & Diener, E. (2000). Personality and subjective well-being across the life span. In V. J. Molfese & D. L. Molfese (Eds.), Temperament and personality development across the life span (pp. 211–234). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marszał-Wiśniewska, M., & Zajusz, D. (2010). Mood as information: The regulatory role of personality. In A. Gruszka, G. Matthews, & B. Szymura (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences in cognition: Attention, memory and executive control (pp. 383–402). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, L. L. (2001). Mood as input: A configural view of mood effects. In L. L. Martin & G. L. Clore (Eds.), Theories of mood and cognition: A user’s guidebook (pp. 135–157). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, L. L., Ward, D. W., Achee, J. W., & Wyer, R. S. (1993). Mood as input: People have to interpret the motivational implications of their moods. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 317–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A. (2004). On the malleability of emotional encoding. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 1019–1036.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2005). Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 167–195.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, E. (2010). Bias in attending to emotional facial expressions: Anxiety and visual search efficiency. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 414–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G., Campbell, S. E., Falconer, S., Joyner, L., Huggins, J., Gilliland, K., et al. (2002). Fundamental dimensions of subjective state in performance settings: Task engagement, distress and worry. Emotion, 2, 315–340.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G., Deary, I. J., & Whiteman, M. C. (2009). Personality traits (3rd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G., Panganiban, A. R., & Hudlicka, E. (2011). Anxiety and selective attention to threat in tactical decision-making. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 949–954.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G., & Wells, A. (2000). Attention, automaticity and affective disorder. Behavior Modification, 24, 69–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, J. D., Allen, J. P., & Beauregard, K. (1995). Mood inductions for four specific moods: A procedure employing guided imagery vignettes with music. Journal of Mental Imagery, 19, 151–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • McFarland, C., Buehler, R., von Ruti, R., Nguyen, L., & Alvaro, C. (2007). The impact of negative moods on self-enhancing cognitions: The role of reflective versus ruminative mood orientations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 728–750.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Attentional bias in generalized anxiety disorder versus depressive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29, 29–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morrone-Strupinsky, J. V., & Lane, D. (2007). Parsing positive emotion in relation to agentic and affiliative components of extraversion. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 1267–1278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nabi, R. L. (2003). Exploring the framing effects of emotion: Do discrete emotions differentially influence information accessibility, information seeking, and policy preference? Communication Research, 30, 224–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nummenmaa, L., & Niemi, P. (2004). Inducing affective states with success-failure manipulations: A meta-analysis. Emotion, 4, 207–214.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Orehek, E., Bessarabova, E., Chen, X., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2011). Positive affect as informational feedback in goal pursuit. Motivation and Emotion, 35, 44–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pedhazur, E. J. (1997). Multiple regression in behavioral research (3rd ed.). Orlando, FL: Harcourt Brace & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simola, J., Kuisma, J., Öörni, A., Uusitalo, L., & Hyn, J. (2011). The impact of salient advertisements on reading and attention on web pages. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17, 174–190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. M., & Petty, E. (1995). Personality moderators of mood congruency effects on cognition: The role of self-esteem and negative mood regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 1092–1107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger, C. D., & Reheiser, E. C. (2004). Measuring anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity as emotional states and personality traits with the STAI, STAXI and STPI. In M. J. Hilsenrothand & D. L. Segal (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of psychological assessment (Vol. 2, pp. 70–86). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Storbeck, J., & Clore, L. (2007). On the interdependence of cognition and emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 1212–1237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trope, Y., Ferguson, M., & Raghunathan, R. (2001). Mood as a resource in processing self-relevant information. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Handbook of affect and social cognition (pp. 256–274). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trope, Y., & Neter, E. (1994). Reconciling competing motives in self-evaluation: The role of self-control in feedback seeking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 646–657.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trope, Y., & Pomerantz, E. M. (1998). Resolving conflicts among self-evaluative motives: Positive experiences as a resource for overcoming defensiveness. Motivation and Emotion, 22, 53–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of Positive and Negative Affect: The PANAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkowski, B. M., Robinson, M. D., Gordon, R. D., & Troop-Gordon, W. (2007). Tracking the evil eye: Trait anger and selective attention within ambiguously hostile scenes. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 650–666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zelenski, J. M., & Larsen, R. J. (1999). Susceptibility to affect: A comparison of three personality taxonomies. Journal of Personality, 67, 761–791.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Almira Kustubayeva gratefully acknowledges support from the Fulbright Visiting Scholar Program in conducting this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gerald Matthews.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kustubayeva, A., Matthews, G. & Panganiban, A.R. Emotion and information search in tactical decision-making: Moderator effects of feedback. Motiv Emot 36, 529–543 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-011-9270-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-011-9270-5

Keywords

Navigation