Skip to main content
Log in

No pain, no change: Reductions in prior negative affect following physical pain

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In general, organisms are motivated to avoid stimuli that induce pain. However, some individuals intentionally inflict pain on themselves (e.g., nonsuicidal self-injury) and report doing so for the perceived emotional benefits following the experience of pain. Two controlled laboratory studies sought to expand upon the relatively limited literature on the effects of pain on emotion. In Study 1, participants provided momentary affect ratings immediately before and after experiencing physical pain. Results demonstrated that both positive affect and negative affect (NA) decreased following the experience of pain. In the Study 2, we examined the effect that individual differences in emotional reactivity had on affective reactions to pain. Individuals high in emotional reactivity experienced larger decreases in NA following the experience of pain than individuals who were low in emotional reactivity. Our findings may potentially explain why some individuals intentionally seek out the experience of pain.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anestis, M. D., Peterson, C. B., Bardone-Cone, A. M., Klein, M. H., Mitchell, J. E., Crosby, R. D., et al. (2009). Affective liability and impulsivity in a clinical sample of women with bulimia nervosa: The role of affect in severely deregulated behavior. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 42, 259–266.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brain, K. L., Haines, J., & Williams, C. L. (2002). The psychophysiology of repetitive self-mutilation. Archives of Suicide Research, 6, 199–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, A. L., Gratz, K. L., & Brown, M. Z. (2006). Solving the puzzle of deliberate self-harm: The experiential avoidance model. Behavior Research and Therapy, 44, 371–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eccleston, C., & Crombez, G. (1999). Pain demands attention: A cognitive-affective model of the interruptive function of pain. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 356–366.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1952). A general introduction to psychoanalysis. New York: Washington Square Press. (Original work published 1920).

    Google Scholar 

  • Haines, J., Williams, C. L., Brain, K. L., & Wilson, G. V. (1995). The psychophysiology of self-mutilation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 471–489.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heath, N. L., Toste, J. R., Nedecheva, T., & Charlebois, A. (2008). An examination of nonsuicual self-injury among college students. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 30, 137–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollin, G. J. S., & Derbyshire, S. W. G. (2009). Cold pressor pain reduces phobic fear but fear does not reduce pain. Journal of Pain, 10, 1058–1064.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klonsky, E. D. (2009). The function of self-injury in young adults who cut themselves: Clarifying the evidence for affect-regulation. Psychiatry Research, 166, 260–268.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leknes, S., Brooks, J. C. W., Wiech, K., & Tracey, I. (2008). Pain relief as an opponent process: A psychophysical investigation. European Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 794–801.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive-behavioral therapy of borderline personality disorder. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mercer, M. E., & Holder, M. D. (1997). Antinociceptive effects of palatable sweet ingesta on human responsivity to pressure pain. Physiology & Behavior, 61, 311–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muehlenkamp, J. J., Engel, S. C., Wadeson, A., Crosby, R. D., Wonderlich, S. A., Simonich, H., et al. (2009). Emotional state preceding and following acts of non-suicidal self-injury in bulimia nervosa patients. Behavior Research and Therapy, 47, 83–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, F. S., & Hagan, B. C. (1973). Pain threshold and tolerance of hands and feet. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 84, 639–643.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nock, M. K., & Prinstein, M. J. (2004). A functional approach to the assessment of self-mutilative behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 885–890.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nock, M. K., Wedig, M. M., Holdberg, E. B., & Hooley, J. M. (2008). The emotion reactivity scale: Development, evaluation and relation to self-injurious thoughts and behaviors. Behavior Therapy, 39, 107–116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Orbach, I., Mikulincer, M., King, R., Cohen, D., & Stein, D. (1997). Thresholds and tolerance of physical pain in suicidal and nonsuicidal adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 646–652.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pomerleau, O. F., Turk, D. C., & Fertig, J. B. (1984). The effects of cigarette smoking on pain and anxiety. Addictive Behaviors, 9, 265–271.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rainville, P., Vet Huynh Boa, Q., & Chrentien, P. (2005). Pain-related emotions modulate experimental pain perception and autonomic responses. Pain, 118, 306–318.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Selby, E. A., Anestis, M. D., Bender, T. W., & Joiner, T. E. (2009). An exploration of the emotional cascade model in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 375–387.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, R. L. (1980). The opponent-process theory of acquired motivation: The costs of pleasure and the benefits of pain. American Psychologist, 35, 691–712.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D. (2000). Mood and temperament. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1994). The PANAS-X: Manual for the positive and negative affect schedule-expanded form. Iowa City: University of Iowa. Unpublished manuscript.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welch, S. S., Linehan, M. M., Sylvers, P., Chittams, J., & Rizvi, S. L. (2008). Emotional responses to self-injury imagery among adults with borderline personality disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 45–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported, in part, by a grant from ND EPSCoR to Kathryn H. Gordon (NSF Grant EPS-081442).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Konrad Bresin.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bresin, K., Gordon, K.H., Bender, T.W. et al. No pain, no change: Reductions in prior negative affect following physical pain. Motiv Emot 34, 280–287 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-010-9168-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-010-9168-7

Keywords

Navigation