Skip to main content
Log in

Abstract Principles and Concrete Cases in Intuitive Lawmaking

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Law and Human Behavior

Abstract

Citizens awaiting jury service were asked a series of items, in Likert format, to determine their endorsement of various statements about principles to use in setting child support amounts. These twenty items were derived from extant child support systems, from past literature and from Ellman and Ellman’s (2008) Theory of Child Support. The twenty items were found to coalesce into four factors (principles). There were pervasive gender differences in respondent’s endorsement of the principles. More importantly, three of these four principles were systematically reflected, in very rational (if complex) ways, in the respondents’ resolution of the individual child support cases they were asked to decide. Differences among respondents in their endorsement of these three principles accounted for differences in their patterns of child support judgments. It is suggested that the pattern of coherent arbitrariness (Ariely et al., Q J Econ 118(1):73–105, 2003) in those support judgments, noted in an earlier study (Ellman, Braver, & MacCoun, 2009) is thus partially explained, in that the seeming arbitrariness of respondents’ initial support judgments reflect in part their differing views about the basic principles that should decide the cases.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We do not here present, analyze or discuss 3 additional Likert items that did not relate explicitly to child support principles. Four of the principles were drawn from Ellman and Ellman (2008), while six others captured views from other publications as well as mother’s and father’s groups. We used twenty items to measure the ten different principles because we intentionally stated most of the principles in more than one format. (E.g., not only a positive but also a negative version, in which disagreement with the statement would indicate agreement with the principle, as well as otherwise identical versions in which reference was, or was not, made to the custodial mother along with the child.).

  2. This procedure discarded the 34 respondents (4%) whose status as support obligor or obligee was gender-atypical, or who had experience as both support obligor and support obligee.

  3. The following analyses were based on the 260 respondents in the Name and Choose conditions who completed both the Likert and the scenario portions of the survey with no missing data. This subset of respondents yields average values for Fig. 1 that are slightly different than the analogous ones reported in Ellman et al. (2009).

  4. Following Aiken and West (1991), these values were computed by inserting into the final equation implied by Table 4 the mean values over all respondents for Dual Obligation and Limiting Father’s Responsibility, while for Gross Disparity Plus we inserted either the mean value (M = 4.92) plus the standard deviation (SD = 1.33), for strong supporters (=6.25), or the mean value minus the standard deviation (=3.59), for weak supporters. “Weak supporters” in this case were thus in mild disagreement with the principle, as the midpoint in the seven-point Likert scale was 4.

References

  • Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical analysis and review of empirical research. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 888–918. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.84.5.888.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ariely, D., Loewenstein, G., & Prelec, D. (2003). Coherent arbitrariness: Stable demand curves without stable preferences. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 118(1), 73–105. doi:10.1162/00335530360535153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 1–62). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braver, S. L., MacCoun, R., & Ellman, I. M. (2008). Converting sentiments to dollars: Scaling and incommensurability problems in the evalution of child support payments. Social Science Research Network, http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=1121240.

  • Bryk, A. S., & Raudenbush, S. W. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (Eds.). (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Current Population Survey. (2006). U.S. Census Bureau, Annual Social and Economic Supplement, Table HINC-01, Selected Characteristics of Households, by Total Money Income in 2005. Retrieved from http://pubdb3.census.gov/macro/032006/hhinc/new01_001.htm

  • Daniels, N. (2008). Reflective equilibrium. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Fall 2008 Edition). Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2008/entries/reflective-equilibrium/

  • Eisenberg, M. A. (1983). The modernization of corporate law: An essay for Bill Cary. University of Miami Law Review, 37(2), 187–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellman, I. M., Braver, S., & MacCoun, R. J. (2009). Intuitive lawmaking: The example of child support. Journal of Empirical Legal Studies, 6(1), 69–109. doi:10.1111/j.1740-1461.2009.01138.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellman, I. M., & Ellman, T. (2008). The theory of child support. Harvard Journal on Legislation, 45(1), 107–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fabricius, W. V., Braver, S. L., Diaz, P., & Velez, C. (2010). Custody and parenting time: Links to family relationships and well-being after divorce. In M.E. Lamb (Ed.), Role of the father in child development (5th ed.) (pp. 201–240). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

  • Floyd, F. J., & Widaman, K. F. (1996). Factor analysis in the development and refinement of clinical assessment instruments. Psychological Assessment, 7(3), 286–299. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.7.3.286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gorsuch, R. L. (1983). Factor analysis (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guadagnoli, E., & Velicer, W. F. (1988). Relation of sample size to the stability of component patterns. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 265–275. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.103.2.265.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guidelines for setting child support awards, 45 C.F.R. § 302.56(c)(2) (2010).

  • Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814–834. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.4.814.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, R. J. (2001). A primer of multivariate statistics (3rd ed.). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Ritov, I., & Schkade, D. (1999). Economic preferences or attitude expressions? An analysis of dollar responses to public issues. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 19, 220–242. doi:10.1023/A:1007835629236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Schkade, D. A., & Sunstein, C. R. (1998). Shared outrage and erratic awards: The psychology of punitive damages. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 16, 49–86. doi:10.1023/A:1007710408413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Legler, P. (2003). Low-income fathers and child support: Starting off on the right track. Denver: Policy Studies Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levi, E. H. (1949). An introduction to legal reasoning. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lichtenstein, S., & Slovic, P. (Eds.). (2006). The construction of preference. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pirog, M., Grieshop, T., & Elliot, B. (2003). Presumptive state child support guidelines: A decade of experience. Policy Currents, 12, 16–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raudenbush, S. W., & Chan, W. S. (1993). Application of hierarchical linear models to study adolescent deviance in an overlapping cohort design. Journal of Clinical and Consulting Psychology, 61(6), 941–951. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.61.6.941.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robbennolt, J. K., Darley, J. M., & MacCoun, R. J. (2003). Symbolism and incommensurability in civil sanctioning: Decision-makers as goal managers, symposium on responsibility and blame: Psychological and legal perspectives. Brooklyn Law Review, 18(4), 1121–1158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robbennolt, J. K., MacCoun, R. J., & Darley, J. M. (2010). Constraint satisfaction and judging. In D. Klein & G. Mitchell (Eds.), The psychology of judicial decision making (pp. 27–40). Oxford. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195367584.003.0002.

  • Simon, D. (2004). A third view of the black box: Cognitive coherence in legal decision making. University of Chicago Law Review, 71, 511–586.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, D., Krawczyk, D. C., & Holyoak, K. J. (2004). Construction of preferences by constraint satisfaction. Psychological Science, 15, 331–336. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00678.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119(1), 3–22. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.119.1.3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stalans, L. J. (1993). Citizens’ crime stereotypes, biased recall, and punishment preferences in abstract cases: The educative role of interpersonal sources. Law and Human Behavior, 17, 451–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (2004). The robot’s rebellion: Finding meaning in the age of Darwin. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Streiner, D. L. (1994). Figuring out factors: The use and misuse of factor analysis. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 39, 135–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sunstein, C., Hastie, R., Payne, J. W., Schkade, D. A., & Viscusi, W. K. (2003). Punitive damages: How juries decide. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, L. L. (1935). The vectors of mind: Multiple-factor analysis for the isolation of primary traits. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Venohr, J., & Griffith, T. E. (2003). Arizona child support guidelines, findings from a case file review. Denver, CO: Policy Studies Inc. Retrieved from http://www.supreme.state.az.us/dr/Pdf/psi2.pdf

  • Wegner, D. M., & Bargh, J. A. (1998). Control and automaticity in social life. In D. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 446–496). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ira Mark Ellman.

About this article

Cite this article

Ellman, I.M., Braver, S.L. & MacCoun, R.J. Abstract Principles and Concrete Cases in Intuitive Lawmaking. Law Hum Behav 1 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-011-9268-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-011-9268-2

Keywords

Navigation