Skip to main content
Log in

(Non-)Arguments in Long-Distance Extractions

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Previous research has shown that in fully grammatical sentences, response time increases and acceptability decreases when the filler in a long-distance extraction is incompatible with the matrix verb. This effect could potentially be due to a difference between argument and adjunct extraction. In this paper we investigate the effect of long extraction of arguments and adjuncts where incompatibility is kept constant. Based on the results from two offline surveys and an online experiment, we argue that the argument/adjunct asymmetry in terms of acceptability is due to differences in processing difficulty, but that both types of extraction involve the same intermediate attachment sites in the online processing.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B. (2011). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999375-42. Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.

  • Chen, E., Gibson, E., & Wolf, F. (2005). Online syntactic storage costs in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 144–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1973). Conditions on transformations. In S. Anderson & P. Kiparsky (Eds.), A festschrift for Morris Halle (pp. 232–286). Rinehart: Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1977). On wh-movement. In P. Culicover, T. Wasow, & A. Akmajian (Eds.), Formal syntax (pp. 71–132). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christensen, K. R., Kizach, J., & Nyvad, A. M. (2013a). Escape from the island: Grammaticality and (reduced) acceptability of wh-island violations in Danish. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 42(1), 51–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christensen, K. R., Kizach, J., & Nyvad, A. M. (2013b). The processing of syntactic islands—An fMRI study. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 26(2), 239–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, S. (1994). WH-agreement and “referentiality” in Chamorro. Linguistic Inquiry, 25, 1–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cinque, G. (1990). Types of A’ dependencies. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diderichsen, P. (1962). Elementær Dansk grammatik. Copenhagen: Gyldendal.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fanselow, G., & Frisch, S. (2006). Effects of processing difficulty on judgments of acceptability. In G. Fanselow, C. Fery, & M. Schlesewsky (Eds.), Gradience in grammar: Generative perspectives (pp. 291–316). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. D. (1978). Parsing strategies and constraints on transformations. Linguistic Inquiry, 9, 427–473.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. D. (1989). Empty categories in sentence processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 3, 155–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K. I., Guerrera, C., & Elliot, L. (2009). The maze task: Measuring forced incremental sentence processing time. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computer, 41, 163–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX. A windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computer, 35, 116–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, L. (1987). Sentence processing: A tutorial review. In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance XII: The psychology of reading (pp. 559–586). East Sussex, U.K.: Erlbaum.

  • Frazier, L., Clifton, C., & Randall, J. (1983). Filling gaps: Decision principles and structure in sentence comprehension. Cognition, 13, 187–222.

  • Garnsey, S. M., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Chapman, R. M. (1989). Evoked potentials and the study of sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18(1), 51–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. (1998). Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 68, 1–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. (2000). The dependency locality theory: A distance-based theory of linguistic complexity. In Y. Miyashita, A. Marantz, & W. O’Neil (Eds.), Image, language, brain (pp. 95–126). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E., & Warren, T. (2004). Reading-time evidence for intermediate linguistic structure in long-distance dependencies. Syntax, 7(1), 55–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins, J. A. (1999). Processing complexity and filler-gap dependencies across grammars. Language, 75(2), 244–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmeister, P. (2007). Representational complexity and memory retrieval in language comprehension. PhD dissertation, Stanford University.

  • Hofmeister, P., & Sag, I. A. (2010). Cognitive constraints and island effects. Language, 86, 366–415.

  • Huang, C. J. (1982). Logical relations in Chinese and the theory of grammar. PhD dissertation, MIT.

  • Hukari, T. E., & Levine, R. D. (1995). Adjunct extraction. Journal of Linguistics, 31, 195–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • King, J., & Just, M. (1991). Who did what and when? Using word- and clausal-level ERPs to monitor working memory usage in reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 376–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kizach, J., Nyvad, A. M., & Christensen, K. R. (2013). Structure before meaning: Sentence processing, plausibility, and subcategorization. PLoS ONE, 8.10, e76326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kluender, R., & Kutas, M. (1993). Bridging the gap: Evidence from ERPs on the processing of unbounded dependencies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 196–214.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey, J. (1979). Transformational syntax and model-theoretic semantics: A case study in Modern Irish. Dordrecht: Reidel.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nicol, J., & Swinney, D. A. (1989). The role of structure in coreference assignment during sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18, 5–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, M. J. (1993). Direct association and sentence processing: A reply to Gorrell and to Gibson and Hickok. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 163–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, M. J., & Traxler, M. J. (2003). Evidence against the use of subcategorisation frequency in the processing of unbounded dependencies. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18, 469–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poole, G. (2011). Syntactic theory (2nd ed.). Houndmills: MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • R Development Core Team. (2009). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from http://www.R-project.org

  • Rizzi, L. (1990). Relativized minimality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprouse, J. (2008). The differential sensitivity of acceptability judgments to processing effects. Linguistic Inquiry, 39(4), 686–694.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stowe, L. A. (1986). Parsing WH-constructions: Evidence for on-line gap location. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1(3), 227–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swinney, D., Ford, M., Frauenfelder, U., & Bresnan, J. (1988). On the temporal course of gap-filling and antecedent assignment during sentence comprehension. In B. Grosz, R. Kaplan, M. Macken, & I. Sag (Eds.), Language structure and processing. Stanford, CA: CSLI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szabolcsi, A. (2006). Strong and weak islands. In M. Everaert, H. van Riemsdijk, R. Goedemans, & B. Hollebrandse (Eds.), The Blackwell companion to syntax (pp. 479–532). London: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Torrego, E. (1984). On Inversion in Spanish and some of its effects. Linguistic Inquiry, 15, 103–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vasishth, S., & Lewis, R. L. (2006). Argument-head distance and processing complexity: Explaining both locality and anti-locality effects. Language, 82, 767–794.

  • Wanner, E., & Maratsos, M. (1978). An ATN approach to comprehension. In N. M. Halle, J. Bresnan, & G. A. Miller (Eds.), Linguistic theory and psychological reality (pp. 119–161). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witzel, N., Witzel, J., & Forster, K. (2012). Comparisons of online reading paradigms: Eye tracking, moving-window, and maze. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 41, 105–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anne Mette Nyvad.

Appendix

Appendix

Experimental stimuli

Long argument movement

Hvilket træ har bestyrelsen vedtaget, at gartneren skal beskære kraftigt?

Which tree has board.the decided that gardener.the must prune rigorously

Hvilken bjergkæde har regeringen tilladt, at forskerne må kortlægge detaljeret?

Which mountain.range has government.the allowed that scientist.the can map detailed

Hvilket sprog har læreren sværget, at eleverne ville tale flydende?

Which language has teacher.the sworn that students.the would speak fluently

Hvilke børn har moderen besluttet, at pædagogerne skulle opdrage strengt?

Which children has mother.the decided that educators.the had.to raise strictly

Hvilken båd har naboen påstået, at præsten ville sælge billigt?

Which boat has neighbor.the claimed that priest.the would sell cheaply

Hvilke aktier har børsmæglerne fortalt, at investorerne ville købe dyrt?

Which stocks have brokers.the told that investors.the would buy expensively

Hvilke udgifter har journalisten hørt, at ledelsen ville nedskære drastisk?

Which expenses has journalist.the heard that management.the would cut.down drastically

Hvilken reol har kunden indrømmet, at flyttemanden skulle transportere langt?

Which book.case has customer.the admitted that mover.the had.to transport long

Long adjunct movement

Hvor kraftigt har bestyrelsen vedtaget, at gartneren skal beskære træet?

How rigorously has board.the decided that gardener.the must prune tree.the

Hvor detaljeret har regeringen tilladt, at forskerne må kortlægge bjergkæden?

How detailed has government.the allowed that scientist.the can map mountain range.the

Hvor flydende har læreren sværget, at eleverne ville tale engelsk?

How fluently has teacher.the sworn that students.the would speak English

Hvor strengt har moderen besluttet, at pædagogerne skulle opdrage børnene?

How strictly has mother.the decided that educators.the had.to raise children.the

Hvor billigt har naboen påstået, at præsten ville sælge båden?

How cheaply has neighbor.the claimed that priest.the would sell boat.the

Hvor dyrt har børsmæglerne fortalt, at investorerne ville købe aktierne?

How expensively have brokers.the told that investors.the would buy stocks.the

Hvor drastisk har journalisten hørt, at ledelsen ville nedskære udgifterne?

How drastically has journalist.the heard that management.the would cut.down expenses.the

Hvor langt har kunden indrømmet, at flyttemanden skulle transportere reolen?

How long has customer.the admitted that mover.the had.to transport book.case.the

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Nyvad, A.M., Kizach, J. & Christensen, K.R. (Non-)Arguments in Long-Distance Extractions. J Psycholinguist Res 44, 519–531 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-014-9300-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-014-9300-z

Keywords

Navigation