Skip to main content
Log in

Government Sanctioned “Tight” and “Loose” Slot Machines: How Having Multiple Versions of the Same Slot Machine Game May Impact Problem Gambling

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Gambling Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In Ontario, Canada, the regulator approves identical looking slot machine games with different payback percentages. We gained access to the design documents (called PAR Sheets) used to program these different versions of the same slots game and ran Gambler’s Ruin simulations of 2,000 first-time players who each arrived with a $100 bankroll and played either the 85 or 98% version of the same game until broke. Simulations revealed that the typical (median) player’s experience did not differ significantly between versions. However the payback percentage affected the experience of players in the upper tails of the distributions with those in the 98% version having dramatically more total spins, winning spins, entries into the “bonus mode”, and “hand pays” (a win of $125 or more on a given spin). Most importantly, the number of simulated players who had a maximum peak balance in excess of $1,000 rose tenfold—from 5 in the 85% version to 54 in the 98% version. The results are discussed in terms of the Pathways Model of Problem and Pathological Gambling especially in terms of behavioural conditioning, cognitive beliefs, and early big wins. It may well be that those machines that are on the surface the “fairest” to the gambler, actually pose the most risk for ensuing gambling problems.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abbott, M. W., & Volberg, R. (1996). The New Zealand national survey of problem and pathological gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 12, 43–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • AGCO. (2007). Alcohol and gaming commission of Ontario: Electronic gaming equipment minimum technical standards. http://www.agco.on.ca/.

  • Blaszczynski, A. (2000). Pathways to pathological gambling: Identifying typologies, eGambling. The Electronic Journal of Gambling Issues, 1, 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blaszczynski, A., & Nower, L. (2002). A pathways model of problem and pathological gambling. Addiction, 97(5), 487–499. May.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coventry, K., & Norman, A. (1998). Arousal, erroneous verbalizations and the illusion of control during a computer-generated gambling task. British Journal of Psychology, 89, 629–645.

    Google Scholar 

  • Custer, R. L. (1984). Profile of the pathological gambler. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 45(2), 35–38.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Custer, R. L., & Milt, H. (1985). When luck runs out. New York: Facts on File.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. (1982). The gambling trap. Psychology Today, 16, 50–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, M. (1990). The cognitive psychology of gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 6, 31–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, M. (1995). Adolescent gambling. London and New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grun, L., & McKeigue, P. (2000). Prevalence of excessive gambling before and after introduction of a national lottery in the United Kingdom: Another example of the single distribution theory. Addiction, 95, 959–966.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harrigan, K., & Dixon, M. (2009). PAR Sheets, probabilities, and slot machine play: Implications for problem and non-problem gambling. Journal of Gambling Issues, 23, 81–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haw, J. (2008). Random-ratio schedules of reinforcement: The role of early wins and unreinforced trials. Journal of Gambling Issues, 21, 56–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kassinove, J., & Schare, M. (2001). Effects of the “near miss” and the “big win” at persistence in slot machine gambling. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15(2), 155–158.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ladouceur, R., & Mayrand, M. (1984). Evaluation of the illusion of control: Type of feedback, outcome sequence, and number of trials among regular and occasional gamblers. The Journal of Psychology, 117, 34–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ladouceur, R., & Walker, M. (1996). A cognitive perspective on gambling. In P. Salkovskis (Ed.), Trends in cognitive and behavioral therapies (pp. 89–120). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, E., & Roth, J. (1975). Heads I win, tails it’s chance: The illusion of control as a function of the sequence of outcomes in a purely chance task. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32(6), 951–955.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Livingtone, C., & Woolley, R. (2008). The relevance and role of gaming machine games and game features on the play of problem gamblers. Report for the independent gaming authority South Australia. Retrieved February 22, 2008, from http://www.iga.sa.gov.au/pdf/0801/Final%20report.Print.Feb08.pdf.

  • Ministry of Public Safety and Solicitor General. (2003). British Columbia problem gambling prevalence study. Final Report, March. Retrieved May 19, 2007, from http://www.bcresponsiblegambling.ca/responsible/bcprobgambstudy.pdf.

  • Moran, E. (1970). Gambling as a form of dependence. British Journal of Addiction, 64, 419–428.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ontario Gaming Control Act. (1992). Ontario regulation 385/99. Amended to O. Reg. 478/01. Games of chance conducted and managed by the Ontario Lottery and Gaming Corporation. http://www.agco.on.ca/en/ft.features/ft4.acts.html. Accessed 21 December 2006.

  • Queensland Office of the Government Statistician & Queensland Gambling Policy Directorate. (2002). Queensland household gambling survey 2001 [electronic resource]: [Web site] Queensland Treasury: Brisbane http://nla.gov.au/nla.arc-51569.

  • Turner, N., & Horbay, R. (2004). How do slot machines and other electronic gambling machines really work? Journal of Gambling Issues, 11, 1–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, N., Zangeneh, M., & Littman-Sharpe, N. (2006). The experience of gambling and its role in problem gambling. International Gambling Studies, 6(2), 237–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Volberg, R. (1996). Gambling and problem gambling in New York: A ten year replication survey, 1986–96. Report to the New York council on problem gambling. Roaring Springs, PA: Gemini Research.

  • Walker, M. (1992). The psychology of gambling. London: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weatherly, J. N., Sauter, J. M., & King, B. M. (2004). The “big win” and resistance to extinction when gambling. The Journal of Psychology, 138, 495–504.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wexler, A. (2002). First person account: Arnie Wexler’s story. Journal of Gambling Issues, 7, 1–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, R., & Wood, R. (2004). Final report to the Ontario problem gambling research centre: The demographic sources of Ontario gaming revenue. Retrieved February 26, 2008, from http://www.gamblingresearch.org.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kevin A. Harrigan.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Harrigan, K.A., Dixon, M. Government Sanctioned “Tight” and “Loose” Slot Machines: How Having Multiple Versions of the Same Slot Machine Game May Impact Problem Gambling. J Gambl Stud 26, 159–174 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-009-9154-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-009-9154-8

Keywords

Navigation