Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Stability in Maternal Autonomy Support and Child Executive Functioning

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Child and Family Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to examine the role of early and current maternal autonomy support, and of its stability over time, in predicting child executive functioning (EF). Seventy-eight mother–child dyads participated in two visits when children were aged 15 months (T1) and 3 years (T2), allowing for the assessment of maternal autonomy support (T1 and T2) and child EF (T2). The results showed that autonomy support at 15 months and the average level of autonomy support displayed by the mothers between 15 months and 3 years were significant predictors of child EF, whereas current autonomy support was not. Group comparison techniques showed that children of mothers who displayed low autonomy support at both 15 months and 3 years performed the worst on EF. These results speak to the relevance of using multiple assessments of parenting behavior when examining its impact on child cognitive development.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Beck, D. M., Schaefer, C., Pang, K., & Carlson, S. M. (2011). Executive function in preschool children: Test-retest reliability. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12, 169–193. doi:10.1080/15248372.2011.563485.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beckwith, L., Rodning, C., & Cohen, S. (1992). Preterm children at early adolescence and continuity and discontinuity in maternal responsiveness from infancy. Child Development, 63, 1198–1208. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01689.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, M. A., & Deater-Deckard, K. (2007). Biological systems and the development of self-regulation: Integrating behavior, genetics, and psychophysiology. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 28, 409–420. doi:10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181131fct.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Belsky, J., Fish, M., & Russell, I. (1991). Continuity and discontinuity in infant negative and positive emotionality: Family antecedents and attachment consequences. Developmental Psychology, 27, 421–431. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.27.3.421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benson, J. E., Sabbagh, M. A., Carlson, S. M., & Zelazo, P. D. (2013). Individual differences in executive functioning predict preschoolers’ improvement from theory-of-mind training. Developmental Psychology, 49, 1615–1627. doi:10.1037/a0031056.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bernier, A., Carlson, S., Deschênes, M., & Matte-Gagné, C. (2012). Social precursors of preschoolers’ executive functioning: A closer look at the early caregiving environment. Developmental Science, 15, 12–24. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01093.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., & Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: Early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning. Child Development, 81, 326–339. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01397.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bernier, A., Matte-Gagné, C., Bélanger, M.-È., & Whipple, N. (2014). Taking stock of two decades of attachment transmission gap: Broadening the assessment of maternal behavior. Child Development,. doi:10.111/cdev.12236.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bibok, M. B., Carpendale, J. I. M., & Müller, U. (2009). Parental scaffolding and the development of executive function. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 123, 17–34. doi:10.1002/cd.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, J., Monuteaux, M. C., Doyle, A. E., Seidman, L. J., Wilens, T. E., Ferrero, F., et al. (2004). Impact of executive function deficits and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on academic outcomes in children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 757–766. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.72.5.757.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of child functioning at school entry. American Psychologist, 57, 111–127. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.57.2.111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, C., & Peters, R. (2003). Physiological and neurocognitive correlates of adaptive behavior in preschool among children in Head Start. Developmental Neuropsychology, 24, 479–497. doi:10.1207/S15326942DN2401_04.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. H. (2002). Parenting infants. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (Vol. 1, pp. 3–43). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. H., Gini, M., Suwalsky, J. T. D., & Leach, D. B. (2006). Emotional availability in mother-child dyads: Short-term stability and continuity from variable-centered and person-centered perspectives. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 52, 547–571. doi:10.1353/mpq.2006.0024.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, R. H., Caldwell, B. M., & Rock, S. L. (1988). Home environment and school performance: A ten-year follow-up and examination of three models of environmental action. Child Development, 59, 852–867. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.ep8589594.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bull, R., Espy, K. A., & Wiebe, S. A. (2008). Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33, 205–228. doi:10.1080/87565640801982312.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, S. M. (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 561–729. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn2802_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, S. M., Mandell, D. J., & Williams, L. (2004). Executive function and theory of mind: Stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 40, 1105–1122. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, A. M., & Clarke, A. D. B. (2000). Early experience and the life path. London: Jessica Kingsley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, C., Prior, M., & Kinsella, G. (2002). The relationship between executive function abilities, adaptive behavior and academic achievement in children with externalizing behavior problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 785–796. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00084.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, E. M., & Bornstein, M. H. (2000). Contemporary research on parenting: The case for nature and nurture. American Psychologist, 55, 218–232. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.2.218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007). Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science, 318, 1387–1388. doi:10.1126/science.1151148.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ellenbogen, M. A., & Hodgins, S. (2004). The impact of high neuroticism in parents on children’s psychosocial functioning in a population at high risk for major affective disorder: A family–environmental pathway of intergenerational risk. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 113–136. doi:10.1017/S0954579404044438.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fraley, R. C., Roisman, G. I., & Haltigan, J. D. (2012). The legacy of early experiences in development: Formalizing alternative models of how early experiences are carried forward over time. Developmental Psychology, 49, 109–126. doi:10.1037/a0027852.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frye, R. E., Malmberg, B., Swank, P., Smith, K., & Landry, S. (2010). Preterm birth and maternal responsiveness during childhood are associated with brain morphology in adolescence. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16, 784–794. doi:10.1017/S1355617710000585.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gerstad, C. L., Hong, Y. J., & Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition and action: Performance of children 3 1/2 –7 years old on a stroop-like day-night test. Cognition, 53, 129–153. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(94)90068-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1989). Parent styles associated with children’s self-regulation and competence in school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 143–154. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.81.2.143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K. E., Fremmer-Bombik, E., Kindler, H., Scheuerer-Englisch, H., & Zimmermann, P. (2002). The uniqueness of the child-father attachment relationship: Fathers’ sensitive and challenging play as a pivotal variable in a 16-year longitudinal study. Social Development, 11, 307–331. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grusec, J. E., & Davidov, M. (2010). Integrating different perspectives on socialization theory and research: A domain-specific approach. Child Development, 81, 687–709. doi:10.111/j.1467-8624.2010.01426.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gutman, L. M., & Feinstein, L. (2010). Parenting behaviours and children’s development from infancy to early childhood: Changes, continuities and contributions. Early Child Development and Care, 180, 535–556. doi:10.1080/03004430802113042.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammond, S. I., Müller, U., Carpendale, J. I. M., Bibok, M. B., & Liebermann-Finestone, D. P. (2012). The effects of parental scaffolding on preschoolers’ executive function. Developmental Psychology, 48, 271–281. doi:10.1037/a0025519.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holden, G. W., & Miller, P. C. (1999). Enduring and different: A meta-analysis of the similarity in parents’ child rearing. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 223–254. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2007). Executive function and theory of mind: Predictive relations from ages 2 to 4. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1447–1459. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1447.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2009). How do families help or hinder the emergence of early executive function? New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 123, 35–50. doi:10.1002/cd.234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., Ensor, R., Wilson, A., & Graham, A. (2010). Tracking executive function across the transition to school: A latent variable approach. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35, 20–36. doi:10.1080/87565640903325691.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J. (1996). Three pleasing ideas. American Psychologist, 51, 901–908.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kochanska, G., & Murray, K. T. (2000). Mother-child mutually responsive orientation and conscience development: From toddler to early school age. Child Development, 71, 417–431. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36, 220–233. doi:10.1037//0012-1649.36.2.220.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lamb, M. E., Thompson, R. A., Gardner, W. P., Charnov, E. L., & Estes, D. (1984). Security of infantile attachment as assessed in the strange situation: Its study and biological interpretation. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 127–171. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00026522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landry, S. H., Miller-Loncar, C. L., Smith, K. E., & Swank, P. R. (2002). The role of early parenting in children’s development of executive processes. Developmental Neuropsychology, 21, 15–41. doi:10.1207/S15326942DN2101_2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., Assel, M. A., & Vellet, S. (2001). Does early responsive parenting have a special importance for children’s development or is consistency across early childhood necessary? Developmental Psychology, 37, 387–403. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.37.3.387.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M. (1997). Altering fate: Why the past does not predict the future. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindhiem, O., Bernard, K., & Dozier, M. (2011). Maternal sensitivity: Within-person variability and the utility of multiple assessments. Child Maltreatment, 16, 41–50. doi:10.1177/1077559510387662.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacCallum, R. C., Zhang, S., Preacher, K. J., & Rucker, D. D. (2002). On the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychological Methods, 7, 19–40. doi:10.1037//1082-989X.7.1.19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mattanah, J. F. (2005). Authoritative parenting and the encouragement of children’s autonomy. In P. A. Cowan, C. P. Cowan, J. C. Ablow, V. K. Johnson, & J. R. Measelle (Eds.), The family context of parenting in children’s adaptation to elementary school (pp. 119–138). Mawah: Erlbuam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matte-Gagné, C., Bernier, A., & Gagné, C. (2013). Stability of maternal autonomy support between infancy and preschool age. Social Development, 22, 427–444. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2012.00667.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayzer, R., Fitzgerald, H. E., & Zucker, R. A. (2009). Anticipating problem drinking risk from preschoolers’ antisocial behavior: Evidence for a common delinquency-related diathesis model. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 820–827. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181aa0383.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2001). Rethinking maternal sensitivity: Mother’s comments on infant’s mental processes predict security of attachment at 12 months. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 637–648. doi:10.1017/S0021963001007302.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moran, G., Forbes, L., Evans, E., Tarabulsy, G. M., & Madigan, S. (2008). Both maternal sensitivity and atypical maternal behavior independently predict attachment security and disorganization in adolescent mother–infant relationships. Infant Behavior and Development, 31, 321–325. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.12.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moses, L. J., & Tahiroglu, D. (2010). Clarifying the relation between executive function and children’s theories of mind. In J. Carpendale, G. Iarocci, U. Müller, B. Sokol, & A. Young (Eds.), Self- and social-regulation: Exploring the relations between social interaction, social cognition, and the development of executive functions (pp. 218–233). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 51–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, M. A., Pien, D. L., & Rothbart, M. K. (1984). Inhibitory self-control in preschool children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 30, 131–147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roisman, G., Collins, W. A., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (2005). Predictors of young adults’ representations of and behavior in their current romantic relationship: Prospective tests of the prototype hypothesis. Attachment & Human Development, 7, 105–121. doi:10.1080/14616730500134928.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schlomer, G. L., Bauman, S., & Card, N. A. (2010). Best practices for missing data management in counseling psychology. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 57, 1–10. doi:10.1037/a0018082.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schulenberg, J. E., Sameroff, A. J., & Cicchetti, D. (2004). The transition to adulthood as a critical juncture in the course of psychopathology and mental health. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 799–806. doi:10.1017/S0954579404040015.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Semrud-Clikeman, M., Walkowiak, J., Wilkinson, A., & Butcher, B. (2010). Executive functioning in children with Asperger syndrome, ADHD-combined type, ADHD-predominately inattentive type, and controls. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 1017–1027. doi:10.1007/s10803-010-0951-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E., & Collins, A. (2005). The development of the person. New York: Guilford. doi:10.1007/s10826-006-9060-y.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., & Kreutzer, T. (1990). The fate of early experience following developmental change: Longitudinal approaches to individual adaptation in childhood. Child Development, 61, 1363–1373. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02867.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • St. Clair-Thompson, H. L., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 745–759. doi:10.1080/17470210500162854.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tarabulsy, G. M., Bernier, A., Provost, M. A., Maranda, J., Larose, S., Moss, E., et al. (2005). Another look inside the gap: Ecological contributions to the transmission of attachment in a sample of adolescent mother-infant dyads. Developmental Psychology, 41, 212–224. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.41.1.212.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vandell, D. L., Belsky, J., Burchinal, M., Steinberg, L., Vandergrift, N., & the NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2010). Do effects of early child care extend to age 15 years? Results from the NICHD study of early child care and youth development. Child Development, 81, 737–756. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01431.x.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, J. H. (1963). Hierarchical grouping to optimize an objective function. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 58, 236–244. doi:10.1080/01621459.1963.10500845.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whipple, N., Bernier, A., & Mageau, G. A. (2011). Broadening the study of infant security of attachment: Maternal autonomy-support in the context of infant exploration. Social Development, 20, 17–32. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2010.00574.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiebe, S. A., Espy, K. A., & Charak, D. (2008). Using confirmatory factor analysis to understand executive control in preschool children: I. Lattent structure. Developmental Psychology, 44, 575–587. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.575.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiebe, S. A., Sheffield, T., Nelson, J. M., Clark, C. A., Chevalier, N., & Espy, K. A. (2011). The structure of executive function in 3-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 436–452. doi:10.1016/j.ecp.2010.08.008.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D. (2006). Implications of 3-yr.-olds’ successful performance on a no conflict version of the dimensional change card sort. Psychological Reports, 98, 858–860. doi:10.2466/pr0.98.3.858-860.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Célia Matte-Gagné.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Matte-Gagné, C., Bernier, A. & Lalonde, G. Stability in Maternal Autonomy Support and Child Executive Functioning. J Child Fam Stud 24, 2610–2619 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-014-0063-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-014-0063-9

Keywords

Navigation