Skip to main content
Log in

Eye-Tracking Measurements of Language Processing: Developmental Differences in Children at High Risk for ASD

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

To explore how being at high risk for autism spectrum disorder (ASD), based on having an older sibling diagnosed with ASD, affects word comprehension and language processing speed, 18-, 24- and 36-month-old children, at high and low risk for ASD were tested in a cross- sectional study, on an eye gaze measure of receptive language that measured how accurately and rapidly the children looked at named target images. There were no significant differences between the high risk ASD group and the low risk control group of 18- and 24-month-olds. However, 36-month-olds in the high risk for ASD group performed significantly worse on the accuracy measure, but not on the speed measure. We propose that the language processing efficiency of the high risk group is not compromised, but other vocabulary acquisition factors might have lead to the high risk 36-month-olds to comprehend significantly fewer nouns on our measure.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. One child contributed data at all three ages and 11 children contributed data at two ages. The total number of children reflects this overlap between the age sub-groups.

  2. Five children contributed data at all three ages and 26 children contributed data at two ages. The total number of children reflects this overlap between the age sub-groups.

  3. The accuracy measure used in this paper is computed using the formula (proportion of time looking at target)/(proportion of time looking at target + proportion of time looking at distractor). This formula by definition does not take into account time spent looking away or lost data for which the eye-tracker outputs an error code.

References

  • Barbaro, J., & Dissanayake, C. (2012). Developmental profiles of infants and toddlers with ASD spectrum disorders identified prospectively in a community-based setting. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 1939–1948. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1441-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bedford, R., Gliga, T., Frame, K., Hudry, K., Chandler, S., Johnson, M. H., & Charman, T. (2013). Failure to learn from feedback underlies word learning difficulties in toddlers at risk for ASD. Journal of Child Language, 40(1), 29–46. doi:10.1017/S0305000912000086.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dale, P. S., & Fenson, L. (1996). Lexical development norms for young children. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28(1), 125–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elsabbagh, M., & Johnson, M. H. (2010). Getting answers from babies about autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(2), 81–87. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • E-Prime Exstensions for Tobii [Computer software]. Sharpsburg, PA: Psychology Software Tools.

  • Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventories: User’s guide and technical manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore: Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ference, J., & Curtin, S. (2013). Attention to lexical stress and early vocabulary growth in 5-month-olds at risk for ASD spectrum disorder. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 891–903. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.08.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2012). Individual differences in lexical processing at 18 months predict vocabulary growth in typically developing and late-talking toddlers. Child Development, 83(1), 203–222. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01692.x.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Hurtado, N. (2008). Input affects uptake: How early language experience influences processing efficiency and vocabulary learning. 7th IEEE international conference on development and learning, 2008. ICDL 2008 (pp. 37–42). doi:10.1109/DEVLRN.2008.4640802

  • Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A. (2013). SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science, 16(2), 234–248. doi:10.1111/desc.12019.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., Perfors, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2006). Picking up speed in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 98–116. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.42.1.98.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., Swingley, D., & Pinto, J. (2001). When half a word is enough: Infants can recognize spoken words using partial acoustic–phonetic information. Child Development, 72, 1003–1015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., Zangl, R., Portillo, A. L., & Marchman, V. A. (2008b). Looking while listening: Using eye movements to monitor spoken language comprehension by infants and young children. In I. A. Sekerina, E. Fernandez, & H. Clahsen (Eds.), Developmental psycholinguistics: On-line methods in children’s language processing (pp. 97–135). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Houston-Price, C., Mather, E., & Sakkalou, E. (2007). Discrepancy between parental reports of infants’ receptive vocabulary and infants’ behaviour in a preferential looking task. Journal of Child Language, 34, 701–724. doi:10.1017/S0305000907008124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hudry, K., Chandler, S., Bedford, R., Pasco, G., Gliga, T., Elsabbagh, M., et al. (2014). Early language profiles in infants at high-risk for autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 154–167.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Landa, R., & Garrett-Mayer, E. (2006). Development in infants with ASD spectrum disorders: A prospective study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 629–638. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01531.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, Edwin H, Jr, Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., & Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223. doi:10.1023/A:1005592401947.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luyster, R. J., Kadlec, M. B., Carter, A., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2008). Language assessment and development in toddlers with ASD spectrum disorders. Journal of ASD and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1426–1438. doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0510-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luyster, R. J., Seery, A., Talbott, M. R., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2011). Identifying early-risk markers and developmental trajectories for language impairment in neurodevelopmental disorders. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 17(2), 151–159. doi:10.1002/ddrr.1109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathematica (Version 9.0) [Computer software]. Champaign, IL:Wolfram.

  • Marchman, V., & Fernald, A. (2008). Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science, 11(3), F9–F16. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00671.x.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Messinger, D., Young, G. S., Ozonoff, S., Dobkins, K., Carter, A., Zwaigenbaum, L., et al. (2013). Beyond ASD: A baby siblings research consortium study of high-risk children at three years of age. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(3), 300–308. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2012.12.011.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, S., Brian, J., Zwaigenbaum, L., Roberts, W., Szatmari, P., Smith, I., et al. (2006). Early language and communication development of infants later diagnosed with ASD spectrum disorder. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27, S69–S78. doi:10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181df7f3c.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning (AGS ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norbury, C. F., Griffiths, H., & Nation, K. (2010). Sound before meaning: Word learning in autistic disorders. Neuropsychologia, 48, 4012–4019. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.10.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Carter, A., Messinger, D., Yirmiya, N., Zwaigenbaum, L., et al. (2011). Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: A Baby Siblings Research Consortium study. Pediatrics, 128(3), e488–e495.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Presmanes, A. G., Walden, T. A., Stone, W. L., & Yoder, P. J. (2007). Effects of different attentional cues on responding to joint attention in younger siblings of children with ASD spectrum disorders. Journal of ASD and Developmental Disorders, 37(1), 133–144. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0338-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynell, J. K., & Grubber, C. P. (1990). Reynell developmental language scale. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social communication questionnaire. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sandin, S., Lichtenstein, P., Kuja-Halkola, R., Larsson, H., Hultman, C. M., & Reichenberg, A. (2014). The familial risk of autism. JAMA, 311(17), 1770–1777.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, W. L., McMahon, C. R., Yoder, P. J., & Walden, T. A. (2007). Early social-communicative and cognitive development of younger siblings of children with ASD spectrum disorders. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 161, 384–390. doi:10.1001/archpedi.161.4.384.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swingley, D., Pinto, J., & Fernald, A. (1999). Continuous processing in word recognition at 24 months. Cognition, 71, 73–108. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00021-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Talbott, M., Nelson, C. A., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2013). Maternal gesture use and language development in infant siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,. doi:10.1007/s1-803-013-1820-0.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tobii Studio [Computer software]. Stockholm, Sweden:Tobii Technology.

  • Toth, K., Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Greenson, J., & Fein, D. (2007). Early social, imitation, play and language abilities of young non-autistic siblings of children with ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 145–157. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0336-2.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Venker, C. E., Eernisse, E. R., Saffran, J. R., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2013). Individual differences in the real-time comprehension of children with ASD. Autism Research,. doi:10.1002/aur.1304.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiig, E. H., Secord, W., & Semel, E. (1992). CELF-preschool: Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals—preschool version. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yirmiya, N., Gamliel, I., Shaked, M., & Sigman, M. (2007). Cognitive and verbal abilities of 24 to 36-month-old siblings of children with ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 218–229. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0163-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, I., Steiner, V., & Pond, R. (2002). Preschool Language Scale (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Rogers, T., Roberts, W., Brian, J., & Szatmari, P. (2005). Behavioral manifestations of ASD in the first year of life. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23, 143–152. doi:10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.05.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We extend special thanks to Max Tegmark for contributions to the data processing and analysis for this study. We are also grateful to Nicole Coman, Anne Seery, Meagan Talbott, Vanessa Vogel and the Nelson lab staff at Children’s Hospital Boston for their help and support and, of course, to all the parents and infants in the Infant Sibling Project who contributed to this research.

Funding

NIH K01DC013306, RO1 DC 10290, Autism Speaks, Simons Foundation.

Conflict of interest

None of the authors have any conflicts of interest.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Meia Chita-Tegmark.

Appendix

Appendix

See Table 3.

Table 3 Noun pairs associated with image pairs

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Chita-Tegmark, M., Arunachalam, S., Nelson, C.A. et al. Eye-Tracking Measurements of Language Processing: Developmental Differences in Children at High Risk for ASD. J Autism Dev Disord 45, 3327–3338 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2495-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2495-5

Keywords

Navigation