Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Validity of the Sluggish Cognitive Tempo, Inattention, and Hyperactivity Symptom Dimensions: Neuropsychological and Psychosocial Correlates

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined the latent structure and validity of inattention, hyperactivity-impulsivity, and sluggish cognitive tempo (SCT) symptomatology. We evaluated mother and teacher ratings of ADHD and SCT symptoms in 140 Puerto Rican children (55.7% males), ages 6 to 11 years, via factor and regression analyses. A three-factor model (inattention, hyperactivity-impulsivity, and SCT) provided the best fit for both sets of ratings. Inattention was the strongest correlate of lower scores on neuropsychological, achievement, and psychosocial measures. Externalizing problems were most strongly associated with hyperactivity-impulsivity, and internalizing problems were most strongly associated with parent-rated SCT and teacher-rated Inattention. SCT was not associated with executive function but was negatively associated with math. Inattention accounted for a disproportionate amount of ADHD-related impairment, which may explain the restricted discriminant validity of DSM-IV types. The distinct factors of hyperactivity-impulsivity and SCT had unique associations with impairing comorbidities and are roughly equivalent in predicting external correlates of ADHD-related impairment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. It can be argued that the conceptual working memory, processing speed, and memory retrieval Indices created require empirically confirmation through factor analysis. We conducted a principal axis factor analysis with varimax rotation and obtained a memory retrieval (MR) factor (variance: 28.65%) defined by loadings on the three measures of the WSRT, and a second factor (variance: 20.34%) that include working memory measures (digits backwards, hands movement, and Simon Task) and RAN measures (digits, colors, and objects). The last factor encompasses two different constructs, working memory and processing speed (WM-PS), that are reported separately in the literature and are conceptually distinct. Nevertheless, we conducted multiple regressions for each of these factors. There was no association between the MR and WM-PS factors and SCT or hyperactivity, respectively, in mothers’ and teachers’ model. Inattention was associated with the MR factor only in the mothers’ model. These findings are consistent with the overall pattern reported in the main text: that relative weakness in neuropsychological performance is chiefly associated with inattention (data available from the corresponding author).

References

  • Achenbach, T. M. (1991a). Manual for the child behavior checklist/4-18, and 1991 profile. Burlington: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry.

    Google Scholar 

  • Achenbach, T. M. (1991b). Manual for the teacher’s report form and 1991 profile. Burlington: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision.). Washington, DC: Author.

  • Baeyens, D., Roeyers, H., & Walle, J. V. (2006). Subtypes of attention-deficit disorder (ADHD): Distinct or related disorders across measurement levels? Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 36, 403–417.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A. (1997). Niños desafiantes [defiant children]. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A. (2011). Distinguishing sluggish cognitive tempo from attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Advance online publication. doi:10.1037/a0023961

  • Barkley, R. A., & Fischer, M. (2011). Predicting impairment in major life activities and occupational functioning in hyperactive children as adults: Self-reported executive function (EF) deficits versus EF tests. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36, 137–161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A., Du Paul, G. J., & McMurray, M. B. (1991). Attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity: Clinical response to three dose levels of methylphenidate. Pediatrics, 87, 519–531.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A., Grodzinsky, G., & Du Paul, G. J. (1992). Frontal lobes function in attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity: A review and research report. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 20, 163–188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A., Edwards, G., Laneri, M., Fletcher, K., & Metevia, L. (2001). Executive functioning, temporal discounting, and sense of time in adolescents with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD). Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 541–557.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bauermeister, J. J. (1994). Development and utilization of the School Behavior Inventory (SBI) for Puerto Rican children. San Juan: Atención, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauermeister, J. J., Matos, M., Reina, G., Salas, C. C., Martínez, J. V., Cumba, E., & Barkley, R. (2005). Comparison of the DSM-IV combined and inattentive types of ADHD in a school-based sample of Latino/Hispanic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 166–179.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bentler, P. M. (2008). EQS 6.1 for windows. Encino: Multivariate Software.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boomsma, A. (2000). Reporting analyses of covariance structures. Structural Equation Modeling, 7, 461–483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breslau, J., Miller, E., Breslau, N., Bohnert, K., Lucia, V., & Schweitzer, J. (2010). The impact of early behavior disturbances on academic achievement in high school. Pediatrics, 123, 1472–1476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, C. L., & Mann, M. (2002). Sluggish cognitive tempo predicts a different pattern of impairment in the attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, predominantly inattentive type. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 31, 123–129.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carr, L., Henderson, J., & Nigg, J. T. (2010). Cognitive control and attentional selection in adolescents with ADHD versus ADD. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 39, 726–740.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chhabildas, N., Pennington, B. F., & Willcutt, E. G. (2001). A comparison of neuropsychological profiles of the DSM-IV subtypes of ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 529–540.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conners, C. K. (1995). Conners’ continuous performance test. Canada: Multi-Health Systems.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. (1974). Rapid “automatized” naming of pictured objects, colors, letters, and numbers by normal children. Cortex, 10, 186–202.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derefinko, K. J., Adams, Z. W., Milich, R., Fillmore, M. T., Lorch, E. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2008). Response style differences in the inattentive and combined subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 745–758.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, T. W., Youngstrom, E. A., Glutting, J. J., & Watkins, M. W. (2007). ADHD and achievement: Meta-analysis of the child, adolescent, and adult literatures and a concomitant study with college students. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 49–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman-Weieneth, J. L., Harvey, E. A., Youngwirth, S. D., & Goldstein, L. H. (2007). The relation between 3-year old children’s skills and their hyperactivity, inattention, and aggression. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 671–681.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garner, A. A., Marceaux, J. C., Mrug, S., Patterson, C., & Hodgens, B. (2010). Dimensions and correlates of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and sluggish cognitive tempo. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 1097–1107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Golden, C. J. (1978). Stroop color and word test: A manual for clinical and experimental use. Chicago: Stoelting.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gresham, F., & Elliott, S. (1990). Social skills rating system. Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, K. M., & Waldman, I. D. (2010). Evaluating the utility of sluggish cognitive tempo in discriminating among DSM-IV ADHD subtypes. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 173–184.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hartman, C. A., Willcutt, E. G., Rhee, S. R., & Pennington, B. F. (2004). The relation between sluggish cognitive tempo and DSM-IV ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32, 491–503.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herrans, L. L., & Rodríguez, J. M. (1992). Manual EIWN-R de Puerto Rico [WISC-R Puerto Rico manual]. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinshaw, S. P., Carte, E. T., Sami, N., Treuting, J. J., & Zupan, B. A. (2002). Preadolescent girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: II. Neuropsychological performance in relation to subtypes and individual classification. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 1099–1111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hofvander, B., Ståhlberg, O., Nydén, A., Wentz, E., Degl'innocenti, A., Billstedt, E., & Anckarsäter, H. (2011). Life history of aggression scores are predicted by childhood hyperactivity, conduct disorder, adult substance abuse, and low cooperativeness in adult psychiatric patients. Psychiatry Research, 185, 280–285.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, L., & Bentler, P. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huang-Pollock, C. L., Nigg, J. T., & Carr, T. H. (2005). Deficient attention is hard to find: Applying the perceptual load model of selective attention to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder subtypes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 1211–1218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (1983). K-ABC interpretive manual. Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lahey, B. B., & Willcutt, E. G. (2010). Predictive validity of a continuous alternative to nominal subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder for DSM-V. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psycholology, 39, 761–775.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lahey, B. B., Pelham, W. E., Schaughency, E. A., Atkins, M. S., Murphy, H. A., Hynd, G. W., & Lorys-Vernon, A. (1988). Dimensions and types of attention deficit disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 330–335.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lahey, B. B., Carlson, C. L., & Frick, P. J. (1997). Attention-deficit disorder without hyperactivity. In T. A. Widiger, A. J. Frances, H. A. Pincus, R. Ross, M. B. First, & W. Davis (Eds.), DSM-IV sourcebook (pp. 163–188). Washington: American Psychiatric.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ludwig, H. T., Matte, B., Katz, B., & Rohde, L. A. (2009). Do sluggish cognitive tempo symptoms predict response to methylphenidate in patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-inattentive type? Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 19, 461–465.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martel, M. M., von Eye, A., & Nigg, J. T. (2010). Revisiting the latent structure of ADHD: Is there a “g” factor? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 905–914.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Massetti, G. M., Lahey, B. B., Pelham, W. E., Loney, J., Ehrhardt, A., Lee, S. S., & Kipp, H. (2008). Academic achievement over 8 years among children who met modified criteria for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder at 4–6 years of age. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 399–410.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matos, B. (2000). A study of the psychometric properties of the Tower of Hanoi, Hand Movements, and Simon tests in a sample of Puerto Rican children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, San Juan, Puerto Rico: Carlos Albizu University.

  • McBurnett, K., Pfiffner, L. J., Willcutt, E., Tamm, L., Lerner, M., Ottolini, Y. L., & Furman, M. A. (1999). Experimental cross-validation of DSM-IV types of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 17–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McBurnett, K., Pfiffner, L. J., & Ottolini, Y. L. (2000). Types of ADHD in DSM-IV. In P. Accardo, T. Blondis, & M. A. Stein (Eds.), The attention deficit disorders (pp. 229–240). New York: Marcel Dekker.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBurnett, K., Pfiffner, L. J., & Frick, P. J. (2001). Symptom properties as a function of ADHD type: An argument for continued study of sluggish cognitive tempo. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 207–213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mikami, A. Y., Huang-Pollock, C. L., Pfiffner, L. J., McBurnett, K., & Hangai, D. (2007). Social skills differences among attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder types in a chat room assessment task. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 509–521.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milich, R., Balentine, A. C., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). ADHD/combined type and ADHD/predominantly inattentive type are distinct and unrelated disorders. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 8, 463–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moran, M. T. (2003). Arguments for rejecting the sequential Bonferroni in ecolological studies. Oikos, 100, 403–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newby, R. (1989). The wisconsin selective reminding test. Milwaukee, WI: Author.

  • Nigg, J. T., Hinshaw, S. P., Carte, E. T., & Treuting, J. J. (1998). Neuropsychological correlates of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder explainable by comorbid disruptive behavior or reading problems? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 468–480.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 51–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., Groisser, D., & Welsh, M. C. (1993). Contrasting cognitive deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder versus reading disability. Developmental Psychology, 29, 511–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Penny, A. M., Wachbusch, D., Klein, R. M., Corkum, P., & Eskes, G. (2009). Developing a measure of sluggish cognitive tempo for children: Content validity, factor structure, and reliability. Psychological Assessment, 21, 380–389.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raykov, T., Tomer, A., & Nesselroade, J. (1991). Reporting structural equation modeling results in psychology and aging: Some proposed guidelines. Psychology and Aging, 6, 499–503.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reeves, C. B., Palmer, S., Gross, A. M., Simonian, S. J., Taylor, L., Willingham, E., & Mulhern, R. L. (2007). Brief report: Sluggish cognitive tempo among pediatric survivors of acute lymphoblastic leukemia. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32, 1050–1054.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitz, M., Ludwig, H., & Rohde, L. A. (2010). Do hyperactive symptoms matter in ADHD-I? Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 39, 741–748.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Lucas, C. P., Dulcan, M. K., & Schwab-Stone, M. E. (2000). NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): Description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 28–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shanahan, M. A., Pennington, B. F., Yerys, B. E., Scott, A., Boada, R., Willcutt, E. G., & DeFries, J. C. (2006). Processing speed deficits in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and reading disability. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 585–602.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skirbekk, B., Hansen, B. H., Oerbeck, B., & Kristensen, H. (2011). The relationship between sluggish cognitive tempo, subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 513–525.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stawicki, J. A., Nigg, J. T., & von Eye, A. (2006). Family psychiatric history evidence on the nosological relations of DSM-IV ADHD combined and inattentive subtypes. New data and meta-analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 935–945.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wahlstedt, C. (2009). Neuropsychological deficits in relation to symptoms of ADHD: Independent contributions and interactions. Child Neuropsychology, 15, 262–279.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wahlstedt, C., & Bohlin, G. (2010). DSM-IV inattention and sluggish cognitive tempo: Independent and interactive relations to neuropsychological factors and comorbidity. Child Neuropsychology, 16, 350–365.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1336–1346.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W. (1982). Batería woodcock psicoeducativa en Español[woodcock Spanish psycho-educational battery]. Allen: DLM Teaching Resources.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Data for this study were obtained through NIMH funded grant: 5R24MH49368-06DHHS and the University of Puerto Rico Institutional Fund for Research. We thank Rafael R. Ramírez, Maribel Matos and our research team for their continuous support.

Financial Disclosure

Dr. José J. Bauermeister was an international consultant to Eli Lilly and receives book royalties from Guilford Publications

Dr. Barkley is a speaker/consultant to Eli Lilly, Shire, Janssen-Ortho, Janssen-Cilag, and Novartis. He has also served as an expert witness for Lilly Canada. He receives product royalties from Guilford Publications, Jones and Bartlett, J & K Seminars, and ContinuingEdOnline.com.

Dr. McBurnett has received research funding, acted as a consultant, and/or served on an advisory board or speaker's bureau for NIMH, Eli Lilly, Lexicor, McNeil Pediatrics, Shire, Abbott Laboratories, Cephalon, Johnson & Johnson, New River, and Sigma-Tau.

Dr. José A. Bauermeister and Dr. Martínez have no financial disclosures to report.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to José J. Bauermeister.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bauermeister, J.J., Barkley, R.A., Bauermeister, J.A. et al. Validity of the Sluggish Cognitive Tempo, Inattention, and Hyperactivity Symptom Dimensions: Neuropsychological and Psychosocial Correlates. J Abnorm Child Psychol 40, 683–697 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011-9602-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011-9602-7

Keywords

Navigation