Skip to main content
Log in

Collective Memory from a Psychological Perspective

  • Published:
International Journal of Politics, Culture, and Society IJPS Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The study of collective memory has burgeoned in the last 20 years, so much so that one can even detect a growing resistance to what some view as the imperialistic march of memory studies across the social sciences (e.g., Berliner 2005; Fabian 1999). Yet despite its clear advance, one area that has remained on the sidelines is psychology. On the one hand, this disinterest is surprising, since memory is of central concern to psychologists. On the other hand, the relative absence of the study of collective memory within the discipline of psychology seems to suit both psychology and other disciplines of the social sciences, for reasons that will be made clear. This paper explores how psychology might step from the sidelines and contribute meaningfully to discussions of collective memory. It reviews aspects of the small literature on the psychology of collective memory and connects this work to the larger scholarly community’s interest in collective memory.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Andersson, J., & Rönnberg, J. (1995). Recall suffers from collaboration: Joint recall effects of friendship and task complexity. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 9, 199–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C. & Levy, B. J. (2002). Inhibitory processes and the control of memory retrieval. Trends in Cognitive Science, 6, 299–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C., Bjork, R. A., & Bjork, E. L. (1994). Remembering can cause forgetting: Retrieval dynamics in long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 20, 1063–1087.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Assmann, A. (1995a). Errinnerungsrdume. Formen und Wandlungen des kulturen Geddchtnisses. Munich, Germany: Beck.

  • Assmann, J. (1995b). Collective memory and cultural identity. New German Critique, 65, 125–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aydede, M., & Robbins, P. (Eds.). (2008). Cambridge handbook of situated cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnier, A. J., Hung, L., & Conway, M. A. (2004). Retrieval-induced forgetting of emotional and unemotional autobiographical memories. Cognition & Emotion, 18, 457–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartlett, F. (1993). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, G. (1979). Mind and nature: A necessary unity (Advances in systems theory, complexity, and the human sciences). New Jersey: Hampton Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beach, K. (1993). Becoming a bartender: the role of external memory cues in a work-directed educational activity. Journal of Applied Cognitive Psychology, 7(3), 191–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berliner, H. (2005). The abuses of memory: reflections on the memory boom in anthropology. Anthropology Quarterly, 78, 197–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bodnar, J. (1992). Remaking America: Public memory, commemoration, and patriotism in the twentieth century. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braun, K. A., Ellis, R., & Loftus, E. F. (2002). Make my memory: how advertising can change our memories of the past. Psychology and Marketing, 19, 1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A., Coman, A. & Hirst, W. (2009). Expertise and the formation of collective memory. Social Psychology, 40, 118–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carley, K. M. (1995). Communication technologies and their effect on cultural homogeneity, consensus, and the diffusion of new ideas. Sociological Perspectives, 38(4), 547–571.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chase, W. G., & Ericsson, K. A. (1982). Skill and working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, (Vol. 16, pp. 1–58). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). The mind's eye in chess. In W. G. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing (pp. 215–281). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ciranni, M. A., & Shimamura, A. P. (1999). Retrieval-induced forgetting in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 25, 1403–1414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. (1998). “The extended mind.” Analysis, 58, 10–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coman, A, Manier, D. & Hirst, W. (2009). Forgetting the unforgettable: socially shared retrieval induced forgetting of September 11 memories. Psychological Science.

  • Cuc, A., Ozuru, Y., Manier, D., & Hirst, W. (2006). The transformation of collective memories: studies of family recounting. Memory & Cognition, 34, 752–762.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cuc, A., Koppel, J., & Hirst, W. (2007). Silence is not golden: a case for socially-shared retrieval-induced forgetting. Psychological Science, 18(2007), 727–737.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dudukovic, N. M., Marsh, E. J., & Tversky, B. (2004). Telling a story or telling it straight: The effects of entertaining versus accurate retellings on memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 125–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, J. M. (2006). Generative social science: studies in agent-based computational modeling. Princeton University Press.

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fabian, J. (1999). Remembering the other: knowledge and recognition in the exploration of Central Africa. Critical Inquiry, 26, 49–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fentress, J., & Wickham, C. (1992). Social memory. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiore, S. M., & Sales, E. (Eds.). (2007). Toward a science of distributed learning. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fivush, R., & Nelson, K. (2004). Culture and language in the emergence of autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 15, 573–577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foucault, M. (1977). Nietzsche, genealogy, history. In D. Bouchard (Ed.), Language, counter-memory, practice: Selected essays and interviews. D. Bouchard, & S. Simon trans. New York: Cornell University Press.

  • Friedkin, N. (1998). A structural theory of social influence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gabbert, F., Memon, A., & Allan, K. (2003). Memory conformity: can eyewitnesses influence each other’s memory for an event? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 533–543.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giddens, A. (1984). The constitution of society: Outline of the theory of structuration. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Givón, T. (2005). Context as other minds. The pragmatics of sociality, cognition and communication. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

  • Hirst, W., & Manier, D. (1996). Social influences on remembering. In D. Rubin (Ed.), Remembering the past (pp. 271–290). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirst, W. & Manier, D. (2008). Towards a psychology of collective memory. Memory, 16, 183–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirst, W., Manier, D., & Apetroaia, I. (1997). The social construction of the remembered self: Family recounting. In J. G. Snodgras & R. L. Thompson (Eds.), The self across psychology. New York: The New York Academy of Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kansteiner, W. (2002). Finding meaning in memory: a methodological critique of collective memory studies. History and Theory, 41, 179–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lea, M. R., & Nicoll, K. (Eds.), (2002). Distributed learning: Social and cultural approaches to practice. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, D. S., Hagen, L., Read, J. D., Wade, K. A., & Garry, M. (2004). True photographs and false memories. Psychological Science, 15, 149–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, E. F. (1993). The reality of repressed memories. American Psychologist, 48, 518–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, E. J. (2007). Retelling is not the same as recalling: implications for memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 16–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazzoni, G. A. L., Loftus, E. F., & Kirsch, I. (2001). Changing beliefs about implausible autobiographical events: a little plausibility goes a long way. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 51–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meade, M. L., & Roediger, H. L. (2002). Explorations in the social cognition of memory. Memory & Cognition, 30, 995–1009.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehl, M. R., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2003). The social dynamics of a cultural upheaval: Social interactions surrounding September 11, 2001. Psychological Science, 14, 579–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Middleton, D., & Edwards, D. (Eds.), (1990). Collective remembering. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. J. (1994). Narrative practices: Their role in socialization and self-construction. In U. Neisser & R. Fivush (Eds.), The remembering self: Construction and accuracy in the self-narrative (pp. 158–179). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, M. E. J, Barabasi, A. L., & Watts, D. J. (Eds.), (1996). The structure and dynamics of complex networks. Princeton University Press, Princeton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olick, J. K. (1999). Collective memory: the two cultures. Sociological Theory, 7, 333–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pasupathi, M., Stallworth, L. M., & Murdoch, K. (1998). How what we tell becomes what we know: listener effects on memory for narratives. Discourse Processes, 26, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L, III, Meade, M. L., & Bergman, E. T. (2001). Social contagion of memory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8, 365–371.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, H. & Shegloff, E. A. & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematic for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language, 50, 696–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salancik, G. R., & Pfeffer, J. (1978). A social information processing approach to job attitudes and task design. Administrative Science Quarterly, 23, 224–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, J., & MacLeod, M. D. (2002). New evidence on the suggestibility of memory: the role of retrieval-induced forgetting in misinformation effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 127–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L. (2001). The seven sins of memory: How the mind forgets and remembers. NY: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schuman, H., Rieger, C., & Gaidys, V. (1994). Collective memories in the United States and Lithuania. In N. Schwarz & S. Sudman (Eds.), Autobiographical memory and the validity of retrospective reports (pp. 313–333). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Cambridge: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stasser, G., & Titus, W. (1985). Pooling of unshared information in group decision making: biased information sampling during discussion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 81–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watts, D. J. (2004). Six degrees: The science of a connected age. NY: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weldon, M. S. (2001) Remembering as a social process. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 10). New York: Academic Press.

  • Wertsch, J. (2002). Voices of collective remembering. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, R. A. (2005). Collective memory, group minds, and the extended mind thesis. Cognitive Processing, 6, 227–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, R. A., & Clark, A. (2008). How to situate cognition: Letting nature takes its course. In P. Robin & M. Aydede (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, D. B., Self, G., & Justice, C. (2000). Memory conformity: Exploring the misinformation effects when presented by another person. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 189–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zerubavel, E. (2006). The elephant in the room: Silence and denial in everyday life. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to William Hirst.

Additional information

The first three authors contributed equally to this paper. The order in which they are listed reflects the throw of a die.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Coman, A., Brown, A.D., Koppel, J. et al. Collective Memory from a Psychological Perspective. Int J Polit Cult Soc 22, 125–141 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10767-009-9057-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10767-009-9057-9

Keywords

Navigation