Skip to main content
Log in

Oral counting sequences: a theoretical discussion and analysis through the lens of representational redescription

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Educational Studies in Mathematics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Empirical research has documented how children’s early counting develops into an increasingly abstract process, and initial counting procedures are reified as children develop and use more sophisticated counting. In this development, the learning of different oral counting sequences that allow children to count in steps bigger than one is seen as an essential skill that supports children’s mental calculation strategies. This paper proposes that the reification or refinement of the counting process that results to increased-in-sophistication use of counting is underlaid by the process of knowledge explicitation that the model of representational redescription postulates. The paper uses a case study to provide insight into the pathway that a 6-year-old child followed from learning how to verbally count in twos and tens to being able to use this knowledge for calculation purposes. The proposal is that knowledge of oral sequences is redescribed in more explicit and accessible formats before children are able to connect their knowledge of the verbal counting with the goal of using the sequence for calculation. The discussion presented here queries the notion of spontaneity as an inherent element of the theory and discusses the role that social interaction may play in supporting knowledge redescription. If it is the case that children’s knowledge of oral counting sequences is redescribed into increasingly explicit formats before it can be applied for calculation, then children need to be provided early in their education with structured activities that trigger knowledge redescription and support the necessary connections between counting, number structure and calculation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anghileri, J. (2008). Uses of counting in multiplication and division. In I. Thompson (Ed.), Teaching and learning early number (pp. 110–121). Maidenhead: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aubrey, C. (2003). “When we were very young”: The foundations for mathematics. In I. Thompson (Ed.), Enhancing primary mathematics teaching (pp. 43–43). Maidenhead: Open University Press.

  • Baroody, A. (1995). The role of the number-after rule in the invention of computational short cuts. Cognition and Instruction, 13, 189–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baroody, A. J., Feil, Y., & Johnson, A. R. (2007). An alternative reconceptualisation of procedural and conceptual knowledge. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 38(2), 115–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baroody, A. J., & Tiilikainen, S. H. (2003). Two perspectives on addition development. In A. J. Baroody & A. Dowker (Eds.), The development of arithmetic concepts and skills (pp. 75–125). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beishuizen, M., & Anghileri, J. (1998). Which mental strategies in the early number curriculum? A comparison of British ideas and Dutch views. British Educational Research Journal, 24(3), 519–538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., & Codd, J. (1997). Cardinal limits: Evidence from language awareness and bilingualism for developing concepts of number. Cognitive Development, 12, 85–106.

  • Bruce, B., & Threlfall, J. (2004). One, two, three and counting. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 55, 3–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (2009). Culture, mind and education. In K. Illeris (Ed.), Contemporary theories of learning. Learning theorists… in their own words (pp. 159–168). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P., & Nunes, T. (2004). Children’s understanding of mathematics. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Childhood cognitive development (pp. 412–439). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2009). Learning and teaching early math. Oxon: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, R. (2003). Does it all add up? Changes in children’s knowledge of addition combinations, strategies and principles. In A. J. Baroody & A. Dowker (Eds.), The development of arithmetic concepts and skills (pp. 35–74). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • DfE (Department for Education). (2013). Mathematics programmes of study: Key stages 1 and 2. National curriculum in England (Report No. DFE-00180-2013). London: Department for Education. Retrieved from https://www.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/335158/PRIMARY_national_curriculum_-_Mathematics_220714.pdf. Accessed 18 Dec 2015.

  • DfES (Department for Education and Skills). (2006). Primary framework for literacy and mathematics. Norwich: DfES.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, J.A., & Bangert, A.S. (2002). The prehistory of discovery: Precursors of representational change in solving gear system problems. Developmental Psychology, 38, 6, 918–933.

  • Dixon, J. A., & Bangert, A. S. (2005). From regularities to concepts: The development of children’s understanding of mathematical relation. Cognitive Development, 20, 65–86.

  • Dowker, A., Bala, S., & Lloyd, D. (2008). Linguistic influences on mathematical development: How important is the transparency of the counting system? Philosophical Psychology, 21(4), 523–538.

  • Ellemor-Collins, D., & Wright, R. (2007). Assessing pupil knowledge of the sequential structure of numbers. Educational and Child Psychology, 24(2), 54–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellemor-Collins, D., & Wright, R. (2011). Developing conceptual place value: Instructional design for intensive intervention. Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties, 16(1), 41–63.

  • Fuson, K. C. (1988). Children’s counting and concepts of number. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fuson, K. C. (1992a). Relationships between counting and cardinality from age 2 to 8. In J. Bideau, C. Meljac, & J.-P. Fisher (Eds.), Pathways to number: Children’s developing numerical abilities (pp. 127–149). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Fuson, K. C. (1992b). Research on whole number addition and subtraction. In D. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 243–275). New York: Macmillan.

  • Gelman, R., & Greeno, J. G. (1989). On the nature of competence: Principles for understanding in a domain. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), Knowing, learning and instruction: Essays in honor of Robert Glaser (pp. 125–186). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

  • Gray, E. (2008). Compressing the counting process: Strengths from the flexible interpretation of symbols. In I. Thompson (Ed.), Teaching and learning early number (pp. 82–93). Maidenhead: Open University Press.

  • Gray, E., & Tall, D. (1992). Success and failure in mathematics: The flexible meaning of symbols as process and concept. Mathematics Teaching, 142, 6–10.

  • Gray, E., & Tall, D. (1994). Duality, ambiguity and flexibility: A proceptual view of simple arithmetic. Journal of Research in Mathematics Education, 26(2), 115–141.

  • Greeno, J. G., Riley, M. S., & Gelman, R. (1984). Conceptual competence and children’s counting. Cognitive Psychology, 16, 94–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, R. (2008). The family counts. In I. Thompson (Ed.), Teaching and learning early number (pp. 47–58). Maidenhead: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1979). Micro- and macro-developmental changes in language acquisition and other representational systems. Cognitive Science, 3, 91–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1984). Children’s problem solving. In M. E. Lamb, A. L. Brown, & B. Rogoff (Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology (pp. 39–90). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. London: The MIT Press.

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1993). What’s special about the development of the human mind/brain? Mind & Language, 8(4), 569–581.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1994). Précis of beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 696–745.

  • LeFevre, J., Clarke, T., & Stringer, A. (2002). Influences of language and parental involvement on the development of counting skills: Comparisons of French- and English-speaking Canadian children. Early Child Development and Care, 172, 283–300.

  • Montague-Smith, A., & Price, A. (2012). Mathematics in early years education. Abington, Oxon: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mulligan, J., & Mitchelmore, M. (2009). Awareness of pattern and structure in early mathematical development. Mathematics Education Research Journal, 21(2), 33–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Munn, P. (2008). Children’s beliefs about counting. In I. Thompson (Ed.), Teaching and learning early number (pp. 19–33). Berkshire: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Resnick, L. (1992). From proroquantities to operators: Building mathematical competence on a foundation of everyday knowledge. In G. Leinhardt, R. Putnam, & R. A. Hattrup (Eds.), Analysis of arithmetic for mathematics teaching (pp. 373–425). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Rittle-Johnson, B., & Koedinger, K. R. (2009). Iterating between lessons on concepts and procedures can improve mathematics knowledge. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 483–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rittle-Johnson, B., Siegler, R. S., & Alibali, M. W. (2001). Developing conceptual understanding and procedural skill in mathematics: An iterative process. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 346–362.

  • Siegler, R. S. (2006). Microgenetic analyses of learning. In D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 464–510). New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S., & Robinson, M. (1982). The development of numerical understandings. In H. W. Reese & L. P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 16, pp. 241–312). London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slavit, D. (1999). The role of operation sense in transitions from arithmetic to algebraic thought. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 37, 251–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sophian, C. (1997). Beyond competence: The significance of performance for conceptual development. Cognitive Development, 12, 281–303.

  • Sophian, C. (2009). Numerical knowledge in early childhood. In R. E. Tremblay, R. G. Barr, R. D. V. Peters, & M. Boivin (Eds.), Encyclopedia of early childhood development (pp. 1–7). Montreal, Quebec: Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development. retrieved from http://www.child-encyclodedia.com/documents/SophianANGxp.pdf. Accessed 23 Nov 2015.

  • Sophian, C., Wood, A., & Vong, K. I. (1995). Making numbers count: The early development of numerical inferences. Developmental Psychology, 31, 263–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Star, R. (2007). Foregrounding procedural knowledge. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 38(2), 132–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Star, R., & Stylianides, G. J. (2013). Procedural and conceptual knowledge: Exploring the gap between knowledge type and knowledge quality. Canadian Journal of Science, Mathematics, and Technology Education, 13(2), 169–181.

  • Steffe, L. P., & Olive, J. (1996). Symbolizing as a constructive activity in a computer microworld. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 14(2), 113–138.

  • Tall, D., Gray, E., Bin Ali, M., Crowley, L., DeMarois, P., McGowen, M., Yusof, Y. (2001). Symbols and the bifurcation between procedural and conceptual thinking. Canadian Journal of Science, Mathematics, and Technology Education, 1(1), 81–104.

  • Thompson, I. (2008). From counting to deriving number facts. In I. Thompson (Ed.), Teaching and learning early number (pp. 97–109). Maidenhead: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, I., & Bramald, R. (2002). An investigation of the relationship between young children’s understanding of the concept of place value and their competence at mental addition. Newcastle upon Tyne, UK: University of Newcastle upon Tyne. Report of the Nuffield Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Threlfall, J. (2008). Development in oral counting, enumeration and counting for cardinality. In I. Thompson (Ed.), Teaching and learning early number (pp. 61–71). Maidenhead: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Threlfall, J., & Bruce, B. (2007). Just counting: Young children’s oral counting and enumeration. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 13(2), 63–77.

  • Voutsina, C. (2012). A micro-developmental approach to studying young children’s problem solving behavior in addition. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 31, 366–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I am thankful to my daughter for sharing with me her knowledge and thoughts about numbers and allowing me to write about our math discussions in this paper. I would like to express my thanks to my colleague Keith Jones for his feedback on an earlier draft of the paper, to the editors and the four anonymous reviewers for their very constructive comments and suggestions on how this work can be taken forward.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Chronoula Voutsina.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Voutsina, C. Oral counting sequences: a theoretical discussion and analysis through the lens of representational redescription. Educ Stud Math 93, 175–193 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-016-9705-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-016-9705-8

Keywords

Navigation