Skip to main content
Log in

Expertise Differences in the Comprehension of Visualizations: a Meta-Analysis of Eye-Tracking Research in Professional Domains

  • Review
  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This meta-analysis integrates 296 effect sizes reported in eye-tracking research on expertise differences in the comprehension of visualizations. Three theories were evaluated: Ericsson and Kintsch’s (Psychol Rev 102:211–245, 1995) theory of long-term working memory, Haider and Frensch’s (J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cognit 25:172–190, 1999) information-reduction hypothesis, and the holistic model of image perception of Kundel et al. (Radiology 242:396–402, 2007). Eye movement and performance data were cumulated from 819 experts, 187 intermediates, and 893 novices. In support of the evaluated theories, experts, when compared with non-experts, had shorter fixation durations, more fixations on task-relevant areas, and fewer fixations on task-redundant areas; experts also had longer saccades and shorter times to first fixate relevant information, owing to superiority in parafoveal processing and selective attention allocation. Eye movements, reaction time, and performance accuracy were moderated by characteristics of visualization (dynamics, realism, dimensionality, modality, and text annotation), task (complexity, time-on-task, and task control), and domain (sports, medicine, transportation, other). These findings are discussed in terms of their implications for theories of visual expertise in professional domains and their significance for the design of learning environments.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Early research on chess masters (Chase and Simon 1973; Jongman 1968 as cited in de Groot and Gobet 1996, Chapter 4) already indicated that extended memory capacities allow experts to recognize more and larger perceptual chunks than novices. These researchers also introduced the idea that chunking at least partly involves recognition (“revisualization”; Jongman 1968) of visual material in long-term memory. Perceptual chunking now constitutes an important perceptual learning mechanism (cf. the concept of unitization, Goldstone 1998).

  2. By visualization, we mean all kinds of pictorial representations of information, including (but not limited to) pictures, graphs, diagrams, concept maps, video films, animations, and simulations.

References

  • *Abernethy, B. (1990). Expertise, visual search, and information pick-up in squash. Perception, 19, 63–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Abernethy, B., & Russel, D. G. (1987). The relationship between expertise and visual search strategy in a racquet sport. Human Movement Science, 6, 283–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Amadieu, F., Van Gog, T., Paas, F., Tricot, A., & Mariné, C. (2009). Effects of prior knowledge and concept-map structure on disorientation, cognitive load, and learning. Learning and Instruction, 19, 376–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Augustyniak, P., & Tadeusiewicz, R. (2006). Assessment of electrocardiogram visual interpretation strategy based on scanpath analysis. Physiological Measurement, 27, 597–608.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Bard, C., & Fleury, M. (1976). Analysis of visual search activity during sport problem situations. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 3, 214–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Bard, C., Fleury, M., Carrière, L., & Hallé, M. (1980). Analysis of gymnastics judges’ visual search. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 51, 267–273.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrouillet, P., & Camos, V. (2007). The time-based resource-sharing model of working memory. In N. Osaka, R. H. Logie, & D. E. Mark (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of working memory (pp. 57–80). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beal, D. J., Corey, D. M., & Dunlap, W. P. (2002). On the bias of Huffcutt and Arthur’s (1995) procedure for identifying outliers in meta-analysis of correlations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 583–589.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Bednarik, R., & Tukiainen, M. (2007). Validating the restricted focus viewer: A study using eye-movement tracking. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 274–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bellenkes, A. H., Wickens, C. D., & Kramer, A. F. (1997). Visual scanning and pilot expertise: The role of attentional flexibility and mental model development. Aviation, Space and Environmental Medicine, 48, 569–579.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Bertrand, C., & Thullier, F. (2009). Effects of player position task complexity in visual exploration behavior in soccer. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 40, 306–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Billett, S. (2009). Personal epistemologies, work, and learning. Educational Research Review, 4, 210–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butcher, K. R. (2006). Learning from text with diagrams: Promoting mental model development and inference generation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 182–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. J. (1986). Task complexity: A review and analysis. Academy of Management Review, 13, 40–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Cauraugh, J. H., Singer, R. N., & Chen, D. (1993). Visual scanning and anticipation of expert and beginning tennis players. In S. Serpa (Ed.), Proceedings of the World Congress of Sport Psychology (pp. 336–340). Lisbon: International Society of Sport Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Chapman, P. R., & Underwood, G. (1998). Visual search of driving situations: Danger and experience. Perception, 27, 951–964.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Chapman, P. R., & Underwood, G. (1999). Visual search of dynamic scenes: Event types and the role of experience in viewing driving situations. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Eye guidance in reading and scene perception (pp. 369–393). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Charness, N., Reingold, E. M., Pomplun, M., & Stampe, D. M. (2001). The perceptual aspect of skilled performance in chess: Evidence from eye movements. Memory & Cognition, 29, 1146–1152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 55–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H. (2006). Two approaches to the study of experts’ characteristics. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 21–30). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Cooper, L., Gale, A., Darker, I., Toms, A., & Saada, J. (2009). Radiology image perception and observer performance: How does expertise and clinical information alter interpretation? Stroke detection explored through eye-tracking. In B. Sahiner & D. J. Manning (Eds.), Medical Imaging 2009: Image perception, observer performance, and technology assessment Vol. 7263 (pp. 72630K–72630K-12). Bellingham: SPIE.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Cooper, L., Gale, A., Saada, J., Gedela, S., Scott, H., & Toms, A. (2010). The assessment of stroke multidimensional CT and MR imaging using eye movement analysis: Does modality preference enhance observer performance? In D. J. Manning & C. K. Abbey (Eds.), Medical Imaging 2010: Image perception, observer performance, and technology assessment Vol. 7627 (pp. 76270B–76270B-12). Bellingham: SPIE.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Crundall, D., Underwood, G., & Chapman, P. (1999). Driving experience and the functional field of view. Perception, 28, 1075–1087.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Crundall, D., Chapman, P., Phelps, N., & Underwood, G. (2003). Eye movements and hazard perception in police pursuit and emergency response driving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 9, 163–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Crundall, D., Chapman, P., France, E., Underwood, G., & Phelps, N. (2005). What attracts attention during police pursuit driving? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 409–420.

    Google Scholar 

  • *de Groot, A. D., & Gobet, F. (1996). Perception and memory in chess. Heuristics of the professional eye. Assen: Van Gorcum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dwyer, F. M., & Joseph, J. (1984). The effects of prior knowledge, presentation mode, and visual realism on student achievement. Journal of Experimental Education, 52, 110–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Dyer, A. G., Found, B., & Rogers, D. (2006). Visual attention and expertise for forensic signature analysis. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 51, 1397–1404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eilam, B., & Poyas, Y. (2008). Learning with multiple representations: Extending multimedia learning beyond the lab. Learning and Instruction, 18, 368–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Lehmann, A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence of maximal adaptations to task constraints. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N., Feltovich, P. J., & Hoffman, R. R. (Eds.). (2006). Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feltovich, P. J., Prietula, M. J., & Ericsson, K. A. (2006). Studies of expertise from psychological perspectives. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 41–67). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1986). The ecological approach to visual perception. New York: Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ginns, P. (2005). Meta-analysis of the modality effect. Learning and Instruction, 15, 313–331.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gnaur, D. (2010). Seizing workplace learning affordances in high-pressure work environments. Vocations and Learning, 3, 223–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone, R. L. (1998). Perceptual learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 585–612.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Goulet, C., Bard, C., & Fleury, M. (1989). Expertise differences in preparing to return a tennis serve: A visual information processing approach. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 11, 382–398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grant, E. R., & Spivey, M. J. (2003). Eye movements and problem solving: Guiding attention guides thought. Psychological Science, 14, 462–466.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haider, H., & Frensch, P. A. (1999). Eye movement during skill acquisition: More evidence for the information reduction hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 25, 172–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, S. M., & Brannick, M. T. (2002). Comparison of two random effects methods of meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 377–389.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Helsen, W., & Pauwels, J. M. (1990). Analysis of visual search activity in solving tactical game problems. In D. Brogan (Ed.), Visual search (pp. 177–184). London: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Helsen, W., & Pauwels, J. M. (1992). A cognitive approach to visual search in sport. In D. Brogan (Ed.), Visual search 2 (pp. 379–388). London: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Helsen, W., & Starkes, J. L. (1999). A multidimensional approach to skilled perception and performance in sport. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13, 1–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, A. J., Alexander, H., Haywood, A., Stapleton, P., Cooke, M., Patterson, E., et al. (2010). Promoting and providing expert guidance in work-intensive clinical settings. Vocations and Learning, 3, 141–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Hermans, O. & Laarni, J. (2003). Searching information from screen maps. Paper presented at the 9th Scandinavian Research Conference on Geographical Information Science, Espoo.

  • Hidi, S. (2006). Interest: A unique motivational variable. Educational Research Review, 1, 69–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Huestegge, L., Skottke, E.-M., Anders, S., Müsseler, J., & Debus, G. (2010). The development of hazard perception: Dissociation of visual orientation and hazard processing. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 13, 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (2004). Methods of meta-analysis: Correcting error and bias in research findings (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyönä, J. (2010). The use of eye movements in the study of multimedia learning. Learning and Instruction, 20, 172–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Jarodzka, H., Scheiter, K., Gerjets, P., & Van Gog, T. (2010). In the eyes of the beholder: How experts and novices interpret dynamic stimuli. Learning and Instruction, 20, 146–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1984). Using eye fixations to study reading comprehension. In D. E. Kieras & M. A. Just (Eds.), New methods in reading comprehension research (pp. 151–182). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S. (2007). Expertise reversal effect and its implications for learner-tailored instruction. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 509–539.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S. (2008). Relative effectiveness of animated and static diagrams: An effect of learner prior knowledge. Computers in Human Behavior, 24, 852–861.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2000). Incorporating learner experience into the design of multimedia instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Kasarskis, P., Stehwien, J., Hickox, J., & Aretz, A. (2001). Comparison of expert and novice scan behaviors during FR flight. Paper presented at the 11th International Symposium on Aviation Psychology, Columbus, OH.

  • *Kato, T., & Fukuda, T. (2002). Visual search strategies of baseball batters: Eye movements during the preparatory phase of batting. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 94, 380–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Konstantopoulos, P. (2009). Investigating drivers’ visual search strategies: Towards an efficient training intervention. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, England.

  • *Kristjanson, A. F., & Antes, J. R. (1989). Eye movement analysis of artists and nonartists viewing paintings. Visual Arts Research, 15, 21–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Krupinski, E. A. (2005). Visual search of mammographic images: Influence of lesion subtlety. Academic Radiology, 12, 965–969.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krupinski, E. A. (2010). Current perspectives in medical image perception. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 1205–1217.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Krupinski, E. A., Tillack, A. A., Richter, L., Henderson, J. T., Bhattacharyya, A. K., Scott, K. M., et al. (2006). Eye-movement study and human performance using telepathology virtual slides. Implications for medical education and differences with experience. Human Pathology, 37, 1543–1556.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D. (2009). Do students need to be taught how to reason? Educational Research Review, 4, 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Kundel, H. L., Nodine, C. F., Conant, E. F., & Weinstein, S. P. (2007). Holistic component of image perception in mammogram interpretation: Gaze-tracking study. Radiology, 242, 396–402.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Laurent, E., Ward, P., Williams, A. M., & Ripoll, H. (2006). Expertise in basketball modifies perceptual discrimination abilities, underlying cognitive processes, and visual behaviours. Visual Cognition, 13, 247–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lesgold, A., Rubinson, H., Feltovich, P., Glaser, R., Klopfer, D., & Wang, Y. (1988). Expertise in a complex domain: Diagnosing X-ray pictures. In M. T. H. Chi, R. Glaser, & M. J. Farr (Eds.), The nature of expertise (pp. 311–342). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Litchfield, D., Ball, L. J., Donovan, T., Manning, D. J., & Crawford, T. (2008). Learning from others: Effects of viewing another person’s eye movements while searching for chest nodules. In B. Sahiner & D. J. Manning (Eds.), Medical Imaging 2008: Image perception, observer performance, and technology assessment Vol. 6917 (pp. 691715–691715-9). Bellingham: SPIE.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Liu X., Gale A. G., & Song, T. (2007). Detection of terrorist threats in air passenger luggage: Expertise development. Paper presented at the 41st Annual IEEE International Carnahan Conference, Ottawa.

  • Mann, D. T. Y., Williams, A. M., Ward, P., & Janelle, C. M. (2007). Perceptual-cognitive expertise in sport: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 29, 457–478.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Manning, D., Ethell, S., Donovan, T., & Crawford, T. (2006). How do radiologists do it? The influence of experience and training on searching for chest nodules. Radiography, 12, 134–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E. (2009). Multimedia learning (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E., & Gallini, J. K. (1990). When is an illustration worth ten thousand words? Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 715–726.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E., & Moreno, R. (2003). Nine ways to reduce cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educational Psychologist, 38, 43–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • *McRobert, A. P., Williams, A. M., Ward, P., & Eccles, D. W. (2009). Tracing the process of expertise in a simulated anticipation task. Ergonomics, 52, 474–483.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., Brooks, R., Shon, A. P., & Rao, R. P. N. (2010). “Social” robots are psychological agents for infants: A test of gaze following. Neural Networks, 23, 966–972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for information processing. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Moran, A., Byrne, A., & McGlade, N. (2002). The effects of anxiety and strategic planning on visual search behaviour. Journal of Sports Sciences, 20, 225–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moreno, R., & Mayer, R. E. (1999). Cognitive principles of multimedia learning: The role of modality and contiguity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 358–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Moreno, F. J., Reina, R., Luis, V., & Sabido, R. (2002). Visual search strategies in experienced and inexperienced gymnastic coaches. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 95, 901–902.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Moreno, F. J., Saavedra, J. M., Sabido, R., Luis, V., & Reina, R. (2006). Visual search strategies of experienced and nonexperienced swimming coaches. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 103, 861–872.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nalanagula, D., Greenstein, J. S., & Gramopadhye, A. K. (2006). Evaluation of the effect of feedforward training displays of search strategy on visual search performance. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 36, 289–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Research Council (1992). Combining information: Statistical issues and opportunities for research. Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Nodine, C. F., Kundel, H. L., Lauver, S. C., & Toto, L. C. (1996). Nature of expertise in searching mammograms for breast masses. Academic Radiology, 3, 1000–1006.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Nodine, C. F., Mello-Thoms, C., Kundel, H. R., & Weinstein, S. P. (2002). Time course of perception and decision making during mammographic interpretation. American Journal of Roentgenology, 179, 917–923.

    Google Scholar 

  • *North, J. S., Williams, A. M., Hodges, N., Ward, P., & Ericsson, K. A. (2009). Perceiving patterns in dynamic action sequences: Investigating the processes underpinning stimulus recognition and anticipation skill. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 878–894.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2003). Cognitive load theory and instructional design: Recent developments. Educational Psychologist, 38, 1–4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plass, J. L., Chun, D. M., Mayer, R. E., & Leutner, D. (2003). Cognitive load in reading a foreign language text with multimedia aids and the influence of verbal and spatial abilities. Computers in Human Behavior, 19, 221–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powers, W. T. (2005). Behavior: The control of perception. New Canaan: Benchmark.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Raab, M., & Johnson, J. G. (2007). Expertise-based differences in search and option-generation strategies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 13, 158–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Reingold, E. M., Charness, N., Pomplun, M., & Stampe, D. M. (2001). Visual span in expert chess players. Psychological Science, 12, 48–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Ripoll, H., Kerlirzin, Y., Stein, J.-F., & Reine, B. (1993). Decision making and visual strategies of boxers in a simulated problem-solving situation. In G. d’Ydewalle & J. Van Rensbergen (Eds.), Perception and cognition: Advances in eye-movement research (pp. 141–147). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Ripoll, H., Kerlirzin, Y., Stein, J.-F., & Reine, B. (1995). Analysis of information processing, decision making, and visual strategies in complex problem solving sport situations. Human Movement Science, 14, 325–349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R., & DiMatteo, M. R. (2001). Meta-analysis: Recent developments in quantitative methods for literature reviews. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 59–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Savelsbergh, G. J. P., Williams, A. M., Van der Kamp, J., & Ward, P. (2002). Visual search, anticipation and expertise in soccer goalkeepers. Journal of Sports Sciences, 20, 279–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Savelsbergh, G. J. P., Van der Kamp, J., Williams, A. M., & Ward, P. (2005). Anticipation and visual search behaviour in expert soccer goalkeepers. Ergonomics, 48, 1686–1697.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheiter, K., Gerjets, P., Huk, T., Imhof, B., & Kammerer, Y. (2009). The effects of realism in learning with dynamic visualizations. Learning and Instruction, 19, 481–494.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, F. L., Oh, I.-S., & Hayes, T. L. (2009). Fixed- versus random-effects models in meta-analysis: Model properties and an empirical comparison of differences in results. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 62, 97–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Schriver, A. T., Morrow, D. G., Wickens, C. D., & Talleur, D. A. (2008). Expertise differences in attentional strategies related to pilot decision making. Human Factors, 50, 864–878.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Singer, R. N., Cauraugh, J. H., Chen, D., Steinberg, G. M., & Frehlich, S. G. (1996). Visual search, anticipation, and reactive comparisons between highly-skilled and beginning tennis players. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 8, 9–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spanjers, I. A. E., Van Gog, T., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2010). A theoretical analysis of how segmentation of dynamic visualizations optimizes students’ learning. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 411–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (1994). Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty, and instructional design. Learning and Instruction, 4, 295–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., Ayres, P., & Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Underwood, G., Crundall, D., & Chapman, P. (2002). Selective searching while driving: The role of experience in hazard detection and general surveillance. Ergonomics, 45, 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Vaeyens, R., Lenoir, M., Williams, A. M., & Philippaerts, R. M. (2007). Mechanisms underpinning successful decision making in skilled youth players: An analysis of visual search behaviors. Journal of Motor Behavior, 39, 395–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Van Gog, T., Paas, F., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2005). Uncovering expertise-related differences in troubleshooting performance: Combining eye movement and concurrent verbal protocol data. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 205–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Vickers, J. N. (1988). Knowledge structures of expert-novice gymnasts. Human Movement Science, 7, 47–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vickers, J. N. (2007). Perception, cognition, and decision training. The quiet eye in action. Champaign: Human Kinetics.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Vogt, S., & Magnussen, S. (2007). Expertise in pictorial perception: Eye-movement patterns and visual memory in artists and laymen. Perception, 36, 91–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Ward, P., Williams, A. M., & Bennett, S. J. (2002). Visual search and biological motion perception in tennis. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 73, 107–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Williams, A. M., & Davids, K. (1997). Assessing cue usage in performance contexts: A comparison between eye-movement and concurrent verbal report methods. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 29, 364–375.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Williams, A. M., & Davids, K. (1998). Visual search strategy, selective attention, and expertise in soccer. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 69, 111–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Williams, A. M., & Elliott, D. (1999). Anxiety, expertise, and visual search strategy in karate. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 21, 362–375.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Williams, A. M., Davids, K., Burwitz, L., & Williams, J. G. (1994). Visual search strategies in experienced and inexperienced soccer players. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 65, 127–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Williams, A. M., Ward, P., Knowles, J. M., & Smeeton, N. J. (2002). Anticipation skill in a real-world task: Measurement, training, and transfer in tennis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 259–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Wilson, M., McGrath, J., Vine, S., Brewer, J., Defriend, D., & Masters, R. (2010). Psychomotor control in a virtual laparoscopic surgery training environment: Gaze control parameters differentiate novices from experts. Surgical Endoscopy, 24, 2458–2464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, R. E. (1986). Task complexity: Definition of the construct. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 37, 60–82.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported in part by grants from the Academy of Finland and from the Doctoral Program for Multidisciplinary Research on Learning Environments (OPMON).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andreas Gegenfurtner.

Additional information

References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis.

Appendix

Appendix

Table 9 Eye-tracking studies of expertise differences in the comprehension of visualizations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gegenfurtner, A., Lehtinen, E. & Säljö, R. Expertise Differences in the Comprehension of Visualizations: a Meta-Analysis of Eye-Tracking Research in Professional Domains. Educ Psychol Rev 23, 523–552 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-011-9174-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-011-9174-7

Keywords

Navigation