Skip to main content
Log in

The influence of the number of relevant causes on the processing of covariation information in causal reasoning

  • Research Report
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Research on causal reasoning has focused on the influence of covariation between candidate causes and effects on causal judgments. We suggest that the type of covariation information to which people attend is affected by the task being performed. For this, we manipulated the test questions for the evaluation of contingency information and observed its influence on both contingency learning and subsequent causal selections. When people select one cause related to an effect, they focus on conditional contingencies assuming the absence of alternative causes. When people select two causes related to an effect, they focus on conditional contingencies assuming the presence of alternative causes. We demonstrated this use of contingency information in four experiments.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Notes

  1. There is a discrepancy in viewpoints within the frequency judgment approach about the use of the conditionalizing strategy in the case of multiple causes. Spellman et al. (2001) suggested that reasoners who are evaluating two causes keep track of three covariations: the covariation between each of the causes and the effect and the covariation between the two causes themselves. In contrast, Cheng suggested that once the subjects have reasons to believe that both factors are potentially causal, they assess each cause only in the absence of the other cause.

  2. There was no significant effect of Treatments in each participant’s choice response, p > 0.3.

  3. There were no significant differences in each of the participants’ predictions, either between ΔP A|~B and ΔP A|~C or between ΔP B|C and ΔP C|B within a session; in all cases, p > 0.2.

  4. There were no differences between the prediction data of Experiments 1 and 1a, and their overall data patterns were very similar: As in the collapsed data, a significant main effect for the condition was observed, but not for the treatment or the interaction between condition and treatment (see Appendix for the details).

References

  • Ahn WK, Kalish CW, Medin DL, Gelman SA (1995) The role of covariation versus mechanism information in causal attribution. Cognition 54(3):299–352

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ahn WK, Kim NS, Lassaline ME, Dennis MJ (2000) Causal status as a determinant of feature centrality. Cogn Psychol 41(4):361–416

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Allan LG, Siegel S, Tangen JM (2005) A signal detection analysis of contingency data. Learn Behav 33(2):250–263

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Allan LG, Hannah SD, Crump MJC, Siegel S (2008) The psychophysics of contingency assessment. J Exp Psychol Gen 137(2):226–243. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.226

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bröder A (2000) Assessing the empirical validity of the “Take-The-Best” heuristic as a model of human probablistic inference. J Exp Psychol 26(5):1332–1346

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheng PW (1997) From covariation to causation: a causal power theory. Psychol Rev 104(2):367–405

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng PW, Novick LR (1990) A probabilistic contrast model of causal induction. J Pers Soc Psychol 58(4):545–567

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng PW, Novick LR (1992) Covariation in natural causal induction. Psychol Rev 99(2):365–382

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng PW, Novick LR (2005) Constraints and nonconstraints in causal learning: Reply to White (2005) and to Luhmann and Ahn (2005). Psychol Rev 112(3):694–706. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.112.3.694

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collins DJ, Shanks DR (2006) Conformity to the power PC theory of causal induction depends on the type of probe question. Q J Exp Psychol 59(2):225–232. doi:10.1080/17470210500370457

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darredeau C, Baetu I, Baker AG, Murphy RA (2009) Competition between multiple causes of a single outcome in causal reasoning. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 35(1):1–14

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fugelsang JA, Thompson VA (2001) Belief-based and covariation-based cues affect causal discounting. Can J Exp Psychol 55(1):70–76

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer G (1997) Bounded rationality: models of fast and frugal inference. Swiss J Econ Stat 133:201–218

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths TL, Tenenbaum JB (2005) Structure and strength in causal induction. Cogn Psychol 51:334–384

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins HM, Ward WC (1965) Judgment of contingency between responses and outcomes. Psychol Monogr Gen Appl 79:1 (Whole No. 594)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klayman J, Ha YW (1987) Confirmation, disconfirmation, and information in hypothesis-testing. Psychol Rev 94(2):211–228

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luhmann CC, Ahn WK (2005) The meaning and computation of causal power: Comment on Cheng (1997) and Novick and Cheng (2004). Psychol Rev 112(3):685–692. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.112.3.685

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Matute H, Miller RR (1996) Cue competition in causal judgment. Int J Psychol 31(3–4):1415

    Google Scholar 

  • Matute H, Arcediano F, Miller RR (1996) Test question modulates cue competition between causes and between effects. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 22(1):182–196

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McKenzie CRM (1994) The accuracy of intuitive judgment strategies: covariation assessment and Bayesian inference. Cogn Psychol 26:209–239

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perales JC, Catena A, Shanks DR, Gonzalez JA (2005) Dissociation between judgments and outcome-expectancy measures in covariation learning: a signal detection theory approach. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 31(5):1105–1120. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.31.5.1105

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shanks DR (2007) Associationism and cognition: human contingency learning at 25. Q J Exp Psychol 60(3):291–309. doi:10.1080/17470210601000581

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spellman BA (1996a) Acting as intuitive scientists: contingency judgments are made while controlling for alternative potential causes. Psychol Sci 7(6):337–342

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spellman BA (1996b) Conditionalizing causality. In: Shanks DR, Holyoak KJ, Medin DL (eds) The psychology of learning and motivation. Vol. 34. Causal learning. Academic, San Diego, pp 167–206

    Google Scholar 

  • Spellman BA, Price CM, Logan JM (2001) How two causes are different from one: the use of (un)conditional information in Simpson’s paradox. Mem Cognit 29(2):193–208

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tangen JM, Allan LG (2003) The relative effect of cue interaction. Q J Exp Psychol B 56(3):279–300

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tangen JM, Allan LG (2004) Cue interaction and judgments of causality: contributions of causal and associative processes. Mem Cognit 32(1):107–124

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vallée-Tourangeau F, Payton T, Murphy RA (2008) The impact of presentation format on causal inferences. Eur J Cognit Psychol 20(1):177–194

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner AR, Logan FA, Haberlandt K (1968) Stimulus selection in animal discrimination learning. J Exp Psychol 76(2 PART 1):171–180

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ward WC, Jenkins HM (1965) The display of information and the judgment of contingency. Can J Psychol 19:231–241

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wasserman EA, Chatlosh DL, Neunaber DJ (1983) Perception of causal relations in humans: factors affecting judgments of response-outcome contingencies under free-operant procedures. Learn Motiv 14(4):406–432

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White PA (1995) Use of prior beliefs in the assignment of causal roles: causal powers versus regularity-based accounts. Mem Cognit 23(2):243–254

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White P (2004) Judgement of two causal candidates from contingency information: effects of relative prevalence of the two causes. Q J Exp Psychol A 57(6):961–991

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White PA (2005) The power PC theory and causal powers: Comment on Cheng (1997) and Novick and Cheng (2004). Psychol Rev 112(3):675–682. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.112.3.675

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winman A, Gredebäk G (2006) Inferring causality assessment from predictive responses: cue interaction without cue competition. Q J Exp Psychol 59(1):28–37

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Jeff Laux for helpful comments on the revision of this article. The authors would also like to thank Serge Blok, John Dennis, and Levi Larkey for discussions about these studies, as well as Jacqueline Alcala, Nathan Janak, Leora Orent, and Youngjun Kim for their help in conducting the experiments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kyungil Kim.

Additional information

Handling editor: Marta Olivetti Belardinelli (Sapienza University of Rome).

Reviewers: Patrice Rusconi (University of Surrey), Simona Sacchi (University of Milano-Bicocca), and a further anonymous reviewer.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 48 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kim, K., Markman, A.B. & Kim, T.H. The influence of the number of relevant causes on the processing of covariation information in causal reasoning. Cogn Process 17, 399–413 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-016-0770-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-016-0770-9

Keywords

Navigation