Skip to main content
Log in

Using a touch screen paradigm to assess the development of mental rotation between 3½ and 5½ years of age

  • Research Report
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Mental rotation is an important spatial skill. However, there is controversy concerning its early development and susceptibility to intervention. In the present study, we assessed individual differences in the mental rotation abilities of children between 3½ and 5½ years of age, using a touch screen paradigm to simplify task demands. A figure or its mirror image was presented in 8 different orientations, and children indicated in which of two holes the figure would fit by touching one of the holes on the screen. Task instructions were varied in three conditions, giving the children the opportunity to gather manual or observational experience with rotations of different stimuli, or giving no additional experience. Children’s error rates and response times increased linearly with increasing angular disparity between the figure and the hole by the age of 5 years, but 4-year-olds were found to respond at chance for all angular disparities, despite the use of a touch screen paradigm. Both manual and observational experience increased the response accuracy of 5-year-olds, especially for children already performing well. However, there was no effect on 4-year-olds. Results point to an emerging readiness to use mental rotation and profit from observational and manual experience at age 5.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Notes

  1. Analyses for 4-year-old good performers showed no effects of disparity, F(4, 32) = 1.31, p = .29, η 2 = 0.14, and no main effect of or interaction with instruction condition (F < 1).

  2. There was no such effect in 4-year-old good performers (F < 1).

References

  • Black T, Schwartz DL (1996) When imagined actions speak louder than words. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Jean Piaget Society, Philadelphia

  • Bruner JS, Olver RR, Greenfield PM (1966) Studies in cognitive growth. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Caldwell EC, Hall VC (1969) The influence of concept training on letter discrimination. Child Dev 40:63–71

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caldwell EC, Hall VC (1970) Concept learning in discrimination tasks. Dev Psychol 2:41–48

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Church RB, Goldin-Meadow S (1986) The mismatch between gesture and speech as an index of transitional knowledge. Cognition 23:43–71

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper LA, Shepard RN (1973) The time required to prepare for a rotated stimulus. Mem Cogn 1:246–250

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson HP (1935) A study of the confusing letters B, D, P and Q. J Genet Psychol 47:458–468

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dean AL, Harvey WO (1979) An information-processing analysis of a Piagetian imagery task. Dev Psychol 15:474–475

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehrlich SB, Levine SC, Goldin-Meadow S (2006) The importance of gesture in children’s spatial reasoning. Dev Psychol 42:1259–1268

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Estes D (1998) Young children’s awareness of their mental activity: the case of mental rotation. Child Dev 69:1345–1360

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frick A, Möhring W (2012) Look, but don’t touch: effects of action and observation experience on infants’ mental object rotation. (Manuscript under review)

  • Frick A, Wang S (in press) Round and round she goes: effects of hands-on training on mental rotation in 13- to 16-month-olds. Child Dev

  • Frick A, Daum MM, Walser S, Mast FW (2009a) Motor processes in children’s mental rotation. J Cogn Dev 10:18–40

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frick A, Daum MM, Wilson M, Wilkening F (2009b) Effects of action on children’s and adults’ mental imagery. J Exp Child Psychol 104:34–51

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Funk M, Brugger P, Wilkening F (2005) Motor processes in children’s imagery: the case of mental rotation of hands. Dev Sci 8:402–408

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson EJ, Pick AD (2000) An ecological approach to perceptual learning and development. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Hespos SJ, Rochat P (1997) Dynamic mental representation in infancy. Cognition 64:153–188

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jahoda G (1979) On the nature of difficulties in spatial-perceptual tasks: ethnic and sex differences. Br J Psychol 70:351–363

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kaess DW (1971) Measures of form constancy: developmental trends. Dev Psychol 4:296

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn SM (1978) The representational-development hypothesis. In: Ornstein PA (ed) Memory development in children. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 157–189

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn SM (1980) Image and mind. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn SM, Margolis JA, Barrett AM, Goldknopf EJ, Daly PF (1990) Age differences in imagery ability. Child Dev 61:995–1010

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Krüger M, Krist H (2009) Imagery and motor processes—when are they connected? The mental transformation of body parts in development. J Cogn Dev 10:239–261

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levine SC, Huttenlocher J, Taylor A, Langrock A (1999) Early sex differences in spatial skills. Dev Psychol 35:940–949

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Linn MC, Petersen AC (1985) Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: a meta-analysis. Child Dev 56:1479–1498

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Marmor GS (1975) Development of kinetic images: when does the child first represent movement in mental images? Cogn Psychol 7:548–559

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marmor GS (1977) Mental rotation and number conservation: are they related? Dev Psychol 13:320–325

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Möhring W, Frick A (in press) Touching up mental rotation: Effects of manual experience on 6-month-old infants’ mental object rotation. Child Dev

  • Moore DS, Johnson SP (2008) Mental rotation in human infants: a sex difference. Psychol Sci 19:1063–1066

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Örnkloo H, von Hofsten C (2007) Fitting objects into holes: on the development of spatial cognition skills. Dev Psychol 43:404–416

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perry M, Church RB, Goldin-Meadow S (1988) Transitional knowledge in the acquisition of concepts. Cogn Dev 3:359–400

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget J (1952) The origins of intelligence in children (trans: Cook M). International Universities Press, New York. (Original work published 1936)

  • Piaget J, Inhelder B (1956) The child’s conception of space (trans: Langdon FJ, Lunzer JL). Routledge and Kegan Paul, London. (Original work published 1948)

  • Piaget J, Inhelder B (1971) Mental imagery in the child; a study of the development of imaginal representation (trans: Chilton PA). Basic, New York. (Original work published 1966)

  • Platt JE, Cohen S (1981) Mental rotation task performance as a function of age and training. J Psychol Interdiscip Appl 108:173–178

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quinn PC, Liben LS (2008) A sex difference in mental rotation in young infants. Psychol Sci 19:1067–1070

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rochat P, Hespos SJ (1996) Tracking and anticipation of invisible spatial transformation by 4- to 8-month-old infants. Cogn Dev 11:3–17

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shea DL, Lubinski D, Benbow CP (2001) Importance of assessing spatial ability in intellectually talented young adolescents: a 20-year longitudinal study. J Educ Psychol 93:604–614

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shepard RN, Cooper LA (1982) Mental images and their transformations. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard RN, Metzler J (1971) Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science 171(3972):701–703

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shutts K, Örnkloo H, von Hofsten C, Keen R, Spelke ES (2009) Young children’s representations of spatial and functional relations between objects. Child Dev 80:1612–1627

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Terlecki MS, Newcombe NS (2005) How important is the digital divide? The relation of computer and videogame usage to gender differences in mental rotation ability. Sex Roles 53:433–441

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Voyer D, Voyer S, Bryden MP (1995) Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: a meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychol Bull 117:250–270

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wai J, Lubinski D, Benbow CP (2009) Spatial ability for STEM domains: aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its importance. J Educ Psychol 101:817–835

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wohlwill JF, Wiener M (1964) Discrimination of form orientation in young children. Child Dev 35:1113–1125

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by research grants from the Swiss National Science Foundation #PBZH1-117012 and from the US National Science Foundation #SBE-0541957.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andrea Frick.

Additional information

This article is part of the special issue on “Spatial Learning and Reasoning Processes”.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Frick, A., Ferrara, K. & Newcombe, N.S. Using a touch screen paradigm to assess the development of mental rotation between 3½ and 5½ years of age. Cogn Process 14, 117–127 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-012-0534-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-012-0534-0

Keywords

Navigation