Skip to main content
Log in

Does social complexity link vocal complexity and cooperation?

  • Review
  • Published:
Journal of Ornithology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In some avian species, individuals spend most of their lives in complex social groups. A recent hypothesis argues that social complexity will drive complexity in signaling systems. According to this hypothesis, individuals living in more complex groups (larger and with greater diversity of interactions) require larger and more diverse repertoires of signals, compared to individuals living in groups that are relatively simple in social structure. Social complexity has also been argued to be an important driver of social cognition and cooperation. Although many of these arguments have been based on empirical findings with non-human primates, similar evidence is beginning to emerge from avian studies. Here, we discuss some of this avian evidence, with an emphasis on two model species: Carolina chickadees, Poecile carolinensis (Paridae), and pied flycatchers, Ficedula hypoleuca. In Carolina chickadees, variation in the structure of chick-a-dee calls influences behavior of receivers in pro-social and potentially cooperative ways in anti-predator and food detection contexts. In pied flycatchers, breeding individuals were much less likely to travel greater distances to assist in mobbing predators near their conspecific neighbors if those neighbors had failed to assist them in mobbing earlier. More research is needed to determine whether communicative complexity per se makes sophisticated social cognition possible, such as reconciliation and cooperation (and whether the latter might stem from reciprocal altruism or less cognitively demanding processes like conspecific by-product mutualism).

Zusammenfassung

Die Individuen einiger Vogelarten verbringen die meiste Zeit ihres Lebens in komplexen sozialen Gruppen. Eine aktuelle Hypothese argumentiert, dass soziale Komplexität die Komplexität in Signalsystemen beeinflusst. Nach dieser Hypothese sollten Individuen, die in komplexeren Gruppen (größer und mit einer größeren Vielfalt von Interaktionen) ein größeres und vielfältigeres Repertoire an Signalen aufweisen im Vergleich zu Individuen, die in Gruppen mit relativ einfacher Sozialstruktur leben. Soziale Komplexität wird auch als ein wichtiger Antriebsfaktor für soziale Kognition und Zusammenarbeit gesehen. Obwohl viele dieser Argumente auf empirischen Ergebnissen von non-humanen Primaten basieren, gibt es Hinweise auf ähnliche Aspekte bei Vögeln. Hier diskutieren wir einige dieser Belege, mit einem Schwerpunkt auf zwei Modellarten: Carolina chickadees, Poecile carolinensis (Paridae) und Trauerschnäpper, Ficedula hypoleuca. Bei Carolina chickadees rufen Veränderung in der Struktur des chick-a-dee Rufes Einflüsse auf das Verhalten der Empfänger hervor, in prosozialer und potenziell kooperativer Weise, z.B. bei der Reaktion gegenüber Prädatoren und im Kontext von Futtererkennung. Bei brütenden Trauerschnäppern waren diese viel weniger bereit und näherten sich nur auf größere Distanzen beim Mobbing von Prädatoren an, um Nachbarn zu helfen, wenn diese Nachbarn sie zuvor beim Mobbing nicht unterstützten. Mehr Forschung ist notwendig, um festzustellen, ob kommunikativen Komplexität an sich erst anspruchsvolle soziale Kognition ermöglicht, wie Versöhnung und Zusammenarbeit (und ob diese möglicherweise von reziprokem Altruismus stammen oder von weniger kognitiv anspruchsvollen Prozessen wie konspezifischen Nebenprodukt-Mutualismus).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barrett L, Henzi P, Rendall D (2007) Social brains, simple minds: does social complexity really require cognitive complexity? Phil Trans R Soc B 362:561–575

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein DT, Armitage KB (1997) Does sociality drive the evolution of communicative complexity? A comparative test with ground-dwelling sciurid alarm calls. Amer Nat 150:179–200

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Boeckle M, Bugnyar T (2012) Long-term memory for affiliates in ravens. Current Biol 22:801–806

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bohn KM, Smarsh GC, Smotherman M (2013) Social context evokes rapid changes in bat song syntax. Anim Behav 85:1485–1491

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bourke AFG (2011) Principles of social evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SL (2011) Principles of animal communication, 2nd edn. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown EC, Brune M (2012) Evolution of social predictive brains? Front Psychol 3:414. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00414

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bshary R, Bergmueller R (2008) Distinguishing four fundamental approaches to the evolution of helping. J Evol Biol 21:405–420

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne RW, Whiten A (eds) (1988) Machiavellian intelligence: social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Carter GG, Wilkinson GS (2013) Food sharing in vampire bats: reciprocal help predicts donations more than relatedness or harassment. Proc R Soc B 280(1753):20122573. doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.2573

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Catchpole CK, Slater PJB (2008) Bird song: biological themes and variations, 2nd edn. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2007) Baboon metaphysics: the evolution of a social mind. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • de Waal FBM, Tyack PL (eds) (2003) Animal social complexity: intelligence, culture, and individualized societies. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • DeScioli P, Kurzban R (2009) The alliance hypothesis for human friendship. PLoS One 4:e5802. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0005802

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Drea CM, Carter AN (2009) Cooperative problem solving in a social carnivore. Anim Behav 78:967–977

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dugatkin LA (1997) Cooperation among animals: an evolutionary perspective. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar RIM (2012) Bridging the bonding gap: the transition from primates to humans. Phil Trans R Soc B 367:1837–1846

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Elgar MA (1986) House sparrows establish foraging flocks by giving chirrup calls if the resources are divisible. Anim Behav 34:169–174

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emery NJ, Seed AM, von Bayern AMP, Clayton NS (2007) Cognitive adaptations of social bonding in birds. Phil Trans R Soc B 362:489–505

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Engh AL, Siebert ER, Greenberg DA, Holekamp KE (2005) Patterns of alliance formation and postconflict aggression indicate spotted hyaenas recognize third-party relationships. Anim Behav 69:209–217

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fraser ON, Bugnyar T (2011) Ravens reconcile after aggressive conflicts with valuable partners. PLoS One 6:e18118. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0018118

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Freeberg TM (2006) Social complexity can drive vocal complexity: group size influences vocal information in Carolina chickadees. Psychol Sci 17:557–561

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freeberg TM, Dunbar RIM, Ord TJ (2012) Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexity Introduction. Phil Trans R Soc B 367:1785–1801

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gingins S, Werminghausen J, Johnstone RA, Grutter AS, Bshary R (2013) Power and temptation cause shifts between exploitation and cooperation in a cleaner wrasse mutualism. Proce R Soc B 280(1761):20130553

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griesser M (2008) Referential calls signal predator behavior in a group-living bird species. Current Biol 18:69–73

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Griesser M (2013) Do warning calls boost survival of signal recipients? Evidence from a field experiment in a group-living bird species. Front Zool 10:49

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Griesser M, Ekman A (2004) Nepotistic alarm calling in the Siberian jay, Perisoreus infaustus. Anim Behav 67:933–939

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey NK (1976) The social function of intellect. In: Bateson PPG, Hinde RA (eds) Growing points in ethology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 303–317

    Google Scholar 

  • Jolly A (1966) Lemur social behavior and primate intelligence. Science 153:501–506

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Koops K, Visalberghi E, van Schaik CP (2014) The ecology of primate material culture. Biol Lett. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2014.0508

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kozlovsky DY, Branch CL, Freas CA, Pravosudov VV (2014) Elevation related differences in novel environment exploration and social dominance in food-caching mountain chickadees. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 68:1871–1881

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kozlovsky DY, Branch CL, Pravosudov VV (2015) Elevation related differences in parental risk taking behavior are associated with cognitive variation in mountain chickadees. Ethology (In Press)

  • Krama T, Vrublevska J, Freeberg TM, Kullberg C, Rantala MJ, Krams I (2012) You mob my owl, I’ll mob yours: birds play tit-for-tat game. Scientific Reports 2:800. doi:10.1038/srep00800

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Krams I, Krama T, Freeberg TM, Kullberg C, Lucas JR (2012) Linking social complexity and vocal complexity: a parid perspective. Phil Trans R Soc B 367:1879–1891

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Krause J, Ruxton GD (2002) Living in groups. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Lestel D, Grundmann E (1999) Tools, techniques and animals: the role of mediations of actions in the dynamics of social behaviours. Soc Sci Inform Sci Soc 38:367–407

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mahurin EJ, Freeberg TM (2009) Chick-a-dee call variation in Carolina chickadees and recruiting flockmates to food. Behav Ecol 20:111–116

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McComb K, Semple S (2005) Coevolution of vocal communication and sociality in primates. Biol Lett 1:381–385

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Melis AP, Tomasello M (2013) Chimpanzees’ (Pan troglodytes) strategic helping in a collaborative task. Biol Letters 9:20130009. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2013.0009

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Micheletta J, Waller BM, Panggur MR, Neumann C, Duboscq J, Agil M, Engelhardt A (2012) Social bonds affect anti-predator behaviour in a tolerant species of macaque, Macaca nigra. Proc R Soc B 279:4042–4050

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Oller DK, Griebel U (eds) (2004) Evolution of communicative systems: a comparative approach. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Oller DK, Griebel U (eds) (2008) Evolution of communicative flexibility: complexity, creativity, and adaptability in human and animal communication. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Page SE (2011) Diversity and complexity. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Pollard KA, Blumstein DT (2012) Evolving communicative complexity: insights from rodents and beyond. Phil Trans R Soc B 367:1869–1878

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Raihani NJ, Bshary R (2011) Resolving the iterated prisoner’s dilemma: theory and reality. J Evol Biol 24:1628–1639

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Raihani NJ, Thornton A, Bshary R (2012) Punishment and cooperation in nature. Trends Ecol Evol 27:288–295

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Randler C, Vollmer C (2013) Asymmetries in commitment in an avian communication network. Naturwissenschaften 100:199–203

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Roush RS, Snowdon CT (1999) The effects of social status on food-associated calling behaviour in captive cotton-top tamarins. Anim Behav 58:1299–1305

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schel AM, Machanda Z, Townsend SW, Zuberbühler K, Slocombe KE (2013) Chimpanzee food calls are directed at specific individuals. Anim Behav 86:955–965

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seed AM, Clayton NS, Emery NJ (2008) Cooperative problem solving in rooks (Corvus frugilegus). Proc R Soc B 275:1421–1429

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2012) The evolutionary origins of friendship. Ann Rev Psychol 63:153–177

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silk JB (2002) Using the ‘F’-Word in primatology. Behaviour 139:421–446

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silk JB (2007) The adaptive value of sociality in mammalian groups. Phil Trans R Soc B 362:539–559

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Silk JB, Alberts SC, Altmann J (2003) Social bonds of female baboons enhance infant survival. Science 302:1231–1234

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki TN (2011) Parental alarm calls warn nestlings about different predatory threats. Current Biol 21:R15–R16

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki TN (2014) Communication about predator type by a bird using discrete, graded and combinatorial variation in alarm calls. Anim Behav 87:59–65

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Templeton CN, Greene E (2007) Nuthatches eavesdrop on variations in heterospecific chickadee mobbing alarm calls. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 104:5479–5482

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Templeton CN, Greene E, Davis K (2005) Allometry of alarm calls: Black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science 308:1934–1937

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Trivers RL (1971) The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q Rev Biol 46:35–57

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson EO (1975) Sociobiology: the new synthesis. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson DR, Mennill DJ (2011) Duty cycle, not signal structure, explains conspecific and heterospecific responses to the calls of Black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus). Behav Ecol 22:784–790

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank the organizers of the IOC for accepting our proposal for a symposium on this topic, and are grateful to the presenters for their wonderful research presentations in the symposium. IK acknowledges the support of a Fulbright Research Award to work at the University of Tennessee during the writing of this manuscript. We thank David Book, Sheri Browning, Brittany Coppinger, Elizabeth Hobson, Amiyaal Ilany, Arik Kershenbaum, Steven Kyle, and two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on earlier drafts of this manuscript. We thank Dr. Christoph Randler for the German translation of our abstract.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Todd M. Freeberg.

Additional information

Communicated by E. Matthysen.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Freeberg, T.M., Krams, I. Does social complexity link vocal complexity and cooperation?. J Ornithol 156 (Suppl 1), 125–132 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10336-015-1233-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10336-015-1233-2

Keywords

Navigation