Skip to main content
Log in

Conspecific alarm cues, but not predator cues alone, determine antipredator behavior of larval southern marbled newts, Triturus pygmaeus

  • Short Communication
  • Published:
acta ethologica Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Predation imposes selection on the ability of prey to recognize and respond to potential threats. Many prey species detect predators via chemoreception, particularly in aquatic environments. Also, chemical cues from injured prey are often perceived as an indication of predation risk. However, because antipredatory behavior can be costly, prey responses should depend on the current level of risk that each predator poses, which may depend on the type of chemical cues detected. We exposed larval newts, Triturus pygmaeus, to chemical cues from predator larval beetles or to alarm cues from conspecific larval newts and examined the behavioral changes of larval newts. Results showed that larval newts reduced activity levels when conspecific alarm cues were present but not when the predator cues alone were present. These results might suggest that larval newts are unable to recognize predator chemicals. To avoid costs of unnecessary antipredatory behaviors, larval newts may benefit by avoiding only predators that represent a current high level of threat, showing only antipredatory responses when they detect conspecific alarm cues indicating that an actual predatory attack has occurred.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

References

  • Adams MJ, Claeson S (1998) Field response of tadpoles to conspecific and heterospecific alarm. Ethology 104:955–962

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amo L, López P, Martín J (2004a) Wall lizards combine chemical and visual cues of ambush snake predators to avoid overestimating risk inside refuges. Anim Behav 67:647–653

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amo L, López P, Martín J (2004b) Chemosensory recognition and behavioral responses of wall lizards, Podarcis muralis, to scents of snakes that pose different risks of predation. Copeia 2004:691–696

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold SJ (1982) A quantitative approach to antipredator performance: salamander defense against snake attack. Copeia 1982:247–253

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold SJ, Wassersug RJ (1978) Differential predation on metamorphic anurans by garter snakes (Thamnophis): social behaviour as a possible defense. Ecology 59:1014–1022

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ASIH (2004) Guidelines for use of live amphibians and reptiles in field and laboratory research. Lawrence, Kansas: Herpetological Animal Care and Use Committee (HACC) of the American Society of Ichthyologists and Herpetologists

  • Boothby KM, Roberts A (1995) Effects of site of tactile stimulation on the escape swimming responses of hatchling Xenopus laevis embryos. J Zool 235:113–125

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brodie ED Jr, Formanowicz DR Jr (1987) Antipredator mechanisms of larval anurans: protection of palatable individuals. Herpetologica 43:369–373

    Google Scholar 

  • Brodie ED Jr, Formanowicz DR Jr, Brodie ED III (1978) The development of noxiousness of Bufo americanus tadpoles to aquatic insect predators. Herpetologica 34:302–306

    Google Scholar 

  • Cabido C, Gonzalo A, Galán P, Martín J, López P (2004) Chemosensory predator recognition induces defensive behavior in the slow-worm (Anguis fragilis). Can J Zool 82:510–515

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chivers DP, Smith RJF (1994) Intra- and interspecific avoidance of areas marked with skin extract from brook stickleback (Culaea inconstans) in a natural habitat. J Chem Ecol 20:1517–1524

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chivers DP, Smith RJF (1998) Chemical alarm signalling in aquatic predator–prey systems: a review and prospectus. Ecoscience 5:338–352

    Google Scholar 

  • Chivers DP, Wisenden BD, Smith RJF (1995) The role of experience in the response of fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas) to skin extract of Iowa darters (Etheostoma exile). Behaviour 132:665–674

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chivers DP, Kiesecker JM, Anderson MT, Wildy EL, Blaustein AR (1996a) Avoidance response of a terrestrial salamander (Ambystoma macrodactylum) to chemical alarm cues. J Chem Ecol 22:1709–1716

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chivers DP, Wisenden BD, Smith RJF (1996b) Damselfly larvae learn to recognize predators from chemical cues in the predator’s diet. Anim Behav 52:315–320

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chivers DP, Kiesecker JM, Marco A, Wildy EL, Blaustein AR (1999) Shifts in life history as a response to predation in western toads (Bufo boreas). J Chem Ecol 25:2455–2463

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dodd CK Jr (1989) Duration of immobility in salamanders, genus Plethodon (Caudata: Plethodontidae). Herpetologica 45:467–472

    Google Scholar 

  • Dodson SI, Crowl TA, Peckarsky BL, Kats LB, Covich AP, Culp JM (1994) Nonvisual communication in fresh-water benthos: an overview. J North Am Benthol Soc 13:268–282

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ducey PK, Brodie ED (1983) Salamanders respond selectively to contacts with snakes: survival advantage of alternative antipredator strategies. Copeia 1983:1036–1041

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eklöv C, Halvarsson H (2000) The trade-off between foraging activity and predation risk for Rana temporaria in different food environments. Can J Zool 78:734–739

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari MCO, Wisenden BD, Chivers DP (2010) Chemical ecology of predator–prey interactions in aquatic ecosystems: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 88:698–724

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Formanowicz DR Jr, Brodie ED Jr (1982) Relative palatabilities of members of a larval amphibian community. Copeia 1982:91–97

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Godin JGJ, Smith SA (1988) A fitness cost of foraging in the guppy. Nature 333:69–71

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gonzalo A, López P, Martín J (2007) Iberian green frog tadpoles may learn to recognize novel predators from chemical alarm cues of conspecifics. Anim Behav 74:447–453

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gonzalo A, López P, Martín J (2009) Learning, memorizing and apparent forgetting of chemical cues from new predators by Iberian green frog tadpoles. Anim Cogn 12:745–750

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gonzalo A, López P, Martín J (2010) Risk level of chemical cues determines retention of recognition of new predators in Iberian green frog tadpoles. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 64:1117–1123

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris RN, Alford RA, Wilbur HM (1988) Density and phenology of Notophthalmus viridescens dorsalis in a natural pond. Herpetologica 44:234–242

    Google Scholar 

  • Helfman GS (1989) Threat-sensitive predator avoidance in damselfish-trumpetfish interactions. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 24:47–58

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hokit DG, Blaustein AR (1995) Predator avoidance and alarm-response behaviour in kin-discriminating tadpoles (Rana cascadae). Ethology 101:280–290

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kats LB, Dill LM (1998) The scent of death: chemosensory assessment of predation risk by prey animals. Ecoscience 5:361–394

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiesecker JM, Chivers DP, Blaustein AR (1996) The use of chemical cues in predator recognition by western toad tadpoles. Anim Behav 52:1237–1245

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laurila A, Karttunen S, Merila J (2002) Adaptive phenotypic plasticity and genetics of larval life histories in two Rana temporaria populations. Evolution 56:617–627

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lawler SP (1989) Behavioural responses to predators and predation risk in four species of larval anurans. Anim Behav 38:1038–1047

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lima SL (1998) Stress and decision making under the risk of predation: recent developments from behavioral, reproductive, and ecological perspectives. Adv Stud Behav 27:215–290

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lima SL, Dill LM (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 68:619–640

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • López P, Martín J (2001) Chemosensory predator recognition induces specific defensive behaviours in a fossorial amphisbaenian. Anim Behav 62:259–264

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maerz JC, Panebianco NL, Madison DM (2001) Effects of predator chemical cues and behavioral biorhythms on foraging activity of terrestrial salamanders. J Chem Ecol 27:1333–1344

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Magurran AE, Irving PW, Henderson PA (1996) Is there a fish alarm pheromone? A wild study and critique. Proc R Soc Lond B 263:1551–1556

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathis A, Smith RJF (1993a) Fathead minnows, Pimephales promelas, learn to recognize northern pike, Esox lucius, as predators on the basis of chemical stimuli from minnows in the pike’s diet. Anim Behav 46:645–656

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathis A, Smith RJF (1993b) Intraspecific and cross-superorder responses to chemical alarm signals by brook stickleback. Ecology 74:2395–2404

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathis A, Vincent F (2000) Differential use of visual and chemical cues in predator recognition and threat-sensitive predator-avoidance responses by larval newts (Notophthalmus viridescens). Can J Zool 78:1646–1652

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Montori A, Herrero P (2004) Triturus pygmaeus (Wolterstorff, 1905). In: Ramos MA (ed) Amphibia, Lissamphhibia. Fauna Iberica vol 24. Museo Nacional de Ciencias Naturales (CSIC), Madrid, pp 221–232

    Google Scholar 

  • Morin PJ (1983) Competitive and predatory interactions in natural and experimental populations of Notophthalmus viridescens dorsalis and Ambystoma tigrinum. Copeia 1983:628–639

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petranka JW, Kats LB, Sih A (1987) Predator–prey interactions among fish and larval amphibians: use of chemical cues to detect predatory fish. Anim Behav 35:420–425

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Polo-Cavia N, Gonzalo A, López P, Martín J (2010) Predator-recognition of native but not invasive turtle predators by naïve anuran tadpoles. Anim Behav 80:461–466

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reques R (2007) Tritón pigmeo—Triturus pygmaeus. In: Carrascal LM., Salvador A (eds) Enciclopedia Virtual de los Vertebrados Españoles. Museo Nacional de Ciencias Naturales, Madrid. Available online at http://www.vertebradosibericos.org/

  • Sih A (1987) Predator and prey lifestyles: an evolutionary and ecological overview. In: Kerfoot WC, Sih A (eds) Predation: direct and indirect impacts on aquatic communities. University Press of New England, Hanover, pp 203–224

    Google Scholar 

  • Sih A, Moore RD (1993) Delayed hatching of salamander eggs in response to enhanced larval predation risk. Am Nat 142:947–960

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Skelly DK (1992) Field evidence for a cost of behavioural antipredator response in a larval amphibian. Ecology 73:704–708

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skelly DK (1994) Activity level and the susceptibility of anuran larvae to predation. Anim Behav 47:465–468

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith RJF (1992) Alarm signals in fishes. Rev Fish Biol Fish 2:3–63

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith RJF (1997) Does one result trump all others? A response to Magurran, Irving and Henderson. Proc R Soc Lond B 264:445–450

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sokal RR, Rohlf FJ (1995) Biometry. WH Freeman, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Stabell OB, Lwin MS (1997) Predator-induced phenotypic changes in crucian carp are caused by chemical signals from conspecifics. Env Biol Fishes 49:145–149

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Storfer A, Sih A (1998) Gene flow and ineffective antipredator behavior in a streambreeding salamander. Evolution 52:558–565

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thoen C, Bauwens D, Verheyen RF (1986) Chemoreceptive and behavioural responses of the common lizard Lacerta vivipara to snake chemical deposits. Anim Behav 34:1805–1813

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tollrian R, Harvell CD (1999) The ecology and evolution of inducible defenses. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson DJ, Lefcort H (1993) The effect of predator diet on the alarm response of red-legged frog, Rana aurora tadpoles. Anim Behav 46:1017–1019

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wisenden BD (2003) Chemically mediated strategies to counter predation. In: Collin SP, Marshall NJ (eds) Sensory assessment of the aquatic environment. Springer, New York, pp 236–251

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Woody DR, Mathis A (1998) Acquired recognition of chemical stimuli from an unfamiliar predator: associative learning by adult newts, Notophthalmus viridescens. Copeia 1998:1027–1031

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments and El Ventorrillo MNCN Field Station for use of their facilities. Financial support was provided by MEC-FPU grants to A.G. and C.C. and by the projects MCI-CGL2008-02119/BOS and MICIIN-CGL2011-24150/BOS.

Integrity of research and reporting

The manuscript submitted for publication complied with the current laws of Spain, and the experiments were carried out under license from the Consejería de Medioambiente de la Comunidad de Madrid (the Environmental Agency of the local Government of Madrid). The authors declare that they have no conflict of interests.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to José Martín.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gonzalo, A., Cabido, C., López, P. et al. Conspecific alarm cues, but not predator cues alone, determine antipredator behavior of larval southern marbled newts, Triturus pygmaeus . acta ethol 15, 211–216 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10211-012-0123-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10211-012-0123-3

Keywords

Navigation