Skip to main content
Log in

Oxytocin enhances the appropriate use of human social cues by the domestic dog (Canis familiaris) in an object choice task

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Animal Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

An Erratum to this article was published on 19 April 2015

Abstract

It has been postulated that the neuropeptide, oxytocin, is involved in human–dog bonding. This may explain why dogs, compared to wolves, are such good performers on object choice tasks, which test their ability to attend to, and use, human social cues in order to find hidden food treats. The objective of this study was to investigate the effect of intranasal oxytocin administration, which is known to increase social cognition in humans, on domestic dogs’ ability to perform such a task. We hypothesised that dogs would perform better on the task after an intranasal treatment of oxytocin. Sixty-two (31 males and 31 females) pet dogs completed the experiment over two different testing sessions, 5–15 days apart. Intranasal oxytocin or a saline control was administered 45 min before each session. All dogs received both treatments in a pseudo-randomised, counterbalanced order. Data were collected as scores out of ten for each of the four blocks of trials in each session. Two blocks of trials were conducted using a momentary distal pointing cue and two using a gazing cue, given by the experimenter. Oxytocin enhanced performance using momentary distal pointing cues, and this enhanced level of performance was maintained over 5–15 days time in the absence of oxytocin. Oxytocin also decreased aversion to gazing cues, in that performance was below chance levels after saline administration but at chance levels after oxytocin administration.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abdi H (2007) The Bonferonni and Šidák corrections for multiple comparisons. In: Salkind N (ed) Encyclopedia of measurement and statistics. Sage Publications Inc, Thousand Oaks

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartz JA, Zaki J, Bolger N, Ochsner KN (2011) Social effects of oxytocin in humans: context and person matter. Trends Cogn Sci 15(7):301–309

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Born J, Lange T, Kern W, McGregor GP, Bickel U, Fehm HL (2002) Sniffing neuropeptides: a transnasal approach to the human brain. Nat Neurosci 5(6):514–516

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brabec CM, Gfeller JD, Ross MJ (2012) An exploration of relationships among measures of social cognition, decision making, and emotional intelligence. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 34(8):887–894

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dal Monte O, Noble PL, Costa VD, Averbeck BB (2014) Oxytocin enhances attention to the eye region in rhesus monkeys. Front Neurosci. doi:10.3389/fnins.2014.00041

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dickstein S, Thompson RA, Estes D, Malkin C, Lamb ME (1984) Social referencing and the security of attachment. Infant Behav Dev 7:507–516

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donges U-S, Kersting A, Suslow T (2012) Women’s greater ability to perceive happy facial emotion automatically: gender differences in affective priming. PLoS ONE 7(8):e41745. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0041745

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Guastella AJ, MacLeod C (2012) A critical review of the influence of oxytocin nasal spray on social cognition in humans: evidence and future directions. Horm Behav 61(3):410–418

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guastella AJ, Mitchell PB, Dadds MR (2008) Oxytocin increases gaze to the eye region of human faces. Biol Psychiatry 63(1):3–5

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Handlin L, Hydbring-Sandberg E, Nilsson A, Ejdebäck M, Jansson A, Uvnäs-Moberg K (2011) Short-term interaction between dogs and their owners: effects of oxytocin, cortisol, insulin and heart-rate—an exploratory study. Anthrozoös 24(3):301–315

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hare B, Brown M, Williamson C, Tomasello M (2002) The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science 298(5598):1634–1636

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hare B, Rosati A, Kaminski J, Bräuer J, Call J, Tomasello M (2010) The domestication hypothesis for dogs’ skills with human communication: a response to Udell et al. (2008) and Wynne et al. (2008). Anim Behav 79:e1–e6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hegedüs D, Bálint A, Miklósi Á, Pongrácz P (2013) Owners fail to influence the choices of dogs in a two-choice, visual pointing task. Behaviour 150:427–443. doi:10.1163/1568539X-00003060

    Google Scholar 

  • IBM Corp. (2013) IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 22.0. IBM Corp., Armonk

    Google Scholar 

  • Kéri S, Benedek G (2009) Oxytocin enhances the perception of biological motion in humans. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 9(3):237–241

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kosfeld M, Heinrichs M, Zak PJ, Fischbacher U, Fehr E (2005) Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature 435(7042):673–676

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lim MM, Young LJ (2006) Neuropeptidergic regulation of affiliative behavior and social bonding in animals. Horm Behav 50(4):506–517

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald E, Dadds MR, Brennan JL, Williams K, Levy F, Cauchi AJ (2011) A review of safety, side-effects and subjective reactions to intranasal oxytocin in human research. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36:1114–1126

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miklósi Á, Polgárdi R, Topál J, Csányi V (1998) Use of experimenter-given cues in dogs. Anim Cogn 1:113–121

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miklósi Á, Kubinyi E, Topál J, Gácsi M, Virányi Z, Csányi V (2003) A simple reason for a big difference: wolves do not look back at humans, but dogs do. Curr Biol 13(9):763–766

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miklósi Á, Pongrácz P, Lakatos G, Topál J, Csányi V (2005) A comparative study of the use of visual communicative signals in interactions between dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans and cats (Felis catus) and humans. J Comp Psychol 119(2):179–186

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller SC, Kennedy C, DeVole D, Hickey M, Nelson T, Kogan L (2009) An examination of changes in oxytocin levels in men and women before and after interaction with a bonded dog. Anthrozoös 22(1):31–42

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagasawa M, Kikusui T, Onaka T, Ohta M (2009) Dog’s gaze at its owner increases owner’s urinary oxytocin during social interaction. Horm Behav 55(3):434–441

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Odendaal JS, Meintjes RA (2003) Neurophysiological correlates of affiliative behaviour between humans and dogs. Vet J 165(3):296–301

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pfungst O (1911) Clever Hans (The horse of Mr. Von Osten): a contribution to experimental animal and human psychology (trans: Rahn CL). Henry Holt and Company, New York

  • Rault J-L (2013) The effects of socio-behavioural contexts on oxytocin in the cerebrospinal fluid of pigs. Proceedings of the Australasia Regional Congress of the International Society for Applied Ethology, Hamilton, New Zealand, p 12

  • Rault J-L, Mack LA, Carter CS, Garner JP, Marchant-Forde JN, Richert BT, Lay DC Jr (2013) Prenatal stress puzzle, the oxytocin piece: prenatal stress alters the behaviour and autonomic regulation in piglets, insights from oxytocin. Appl Anim Behav Sci 148:99–107

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riedel J, Buttelmann D, Call J, Tomasello M (2006) Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use a physical marker to locate hidden food. Anim Cogn 9(1):27–35

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Riedel J, Schumann K, Kaminski J, Call J, Tomasello M (2008) The early ontogeny of human-dog communication. Anim Behav 75:1003–1014

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rissman EF (2008) Roles of oestrogen receptors alpha and beta in behavioural neuroendocrinology: beyond Yin/Yang. J Neuroendocrinol 20(6):873–879

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidjell T, Range F, Huber L, Virányi Z (2012) Do owners have a Clever Hans effect on dogs? Results of a pointing study. Front Psychol. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00558

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Smith BP, Litchfield CA (2010) Dingoes (Canis dingo) can use human social cues to locate hidden food. Anim Cogn 13(2):367–376

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Soproni K, Miklósi Á, Topál J, Csányi V (2001) Comprehension of human communicative signs in pet dogs (Canis familiaris). J Comp Psychol 115(2):122–126

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Soproni K, Miklósi Á, Topál J, Csányi V (2002) Dogs’ (Canis familiaris) responsiveness to human pointing gestures. J Comp Psychol 116(1):27–34

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Striano T, Vaish A, Benigno JP (2006) The meaning of infants’ looks: information seeking and comfort seeking? Br J Dev Psychol 24:615–630

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Topál J, Miklósi Á, Csányi V (1997) Dog-human relationship affects problem solving behavior in the dog. Anthrozoös 10(4):214–224

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Udell MAR, Wynne CDL (2010) Ontogeny and phylogeny: both are essential to human-sensitive behaviour in the genus Canis. Anim Behav 79:e9–e14

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Udell MA, Giglio RF, Wynne CD (2008a) Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use human gestures but not nonhuman tokens to find hidden food. J Comp Psychol 122(1):84–93

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Udell MAR, Dorey NR, Wynne CDL (2008b) Wolves outperform dogs in following human social cues. Anim Behav 76:1767–1773

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Virányi Z, Gácsi M, Kubinyi E, Topál J, Belényi B, Ujfalussy D, Miklósi Á (2008) Comprehension of human pointing gestures in young human-reared wolves (Canis lupus) and dogs (Canis familiaris). Anim Cogn 11(3):373–387

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten A, Erdal D (2012) The human socio-cognitive niche and its evolutionary origins. Philos Trans R Soc B 367(1599):2119–2129

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wobber V, Hare B, Koler-Matznick J, Wrangham R, Tomasello M (2009) Breed differences in domestic dogs’ (Canis familiaris) comprehension of human communicative signals. Interact Stud 10(2):206–224

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wynne CDL, Udell MAR, Lord KA (2008) Ontogeny’s impacts on human-dog communication. Anim Behav 76:e1–e4

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the Clear Dog Shop and Love’ em for sponsoring the study and providing dog food rewards. We would also like to thank Lachlan Macquarie, Department of Econometrics and Business Statistics, Monash University, for his statistical advice.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to J. L. Oliva.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Oliva, J.L., Rault, JL., Appleton, B. et al. Oxytocin enhances the appropriate use of human social cues by the domestic dog (Canis familiaris) in an object choice task. Anim Cogn 18, 767–775 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0843-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0843-7

Keywords

Navigation