Skip to main content
Log in

How visual working memory contents influence priming of visual attention

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Recent evidence shows that when the contents of visual working memory overlap with targets and distractors in a pop-out search task, intertrial priming is inhibited (Kristjánsson, Sævarsson & Driver, Psychon Bull Rev 20(3):514–521, 2013, Experiment 2, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review). This may reflect an interesting interaction between implicit short-term memory—thought to underlie intertrial priming—and explicit visual working memory. Evidence from a non-pop-out search task suggests that it may specifically be holding distractors in visual working memory that disrupts intertrial priming (Cunningham & Egeth, Psychol Sci 27(4):476–485, 2016, Experiment 2, Psychological Science). We examined whether the inhibition of priming depends on whether feature values in visual working memory overlap with targets or distractors in the pop-out search, and we found that the inhibition of priming resulted from holding distractors in visual working memory. These results are consistent with separate mechanisms of target and distractor effects in intertrial priming, and support the notion that the impact of implicit short-term memory and explicit visual working memory can interact when each provides conflicting attentional signals.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. A′ is derived from the more traditional d′ but is more accurate for tasks of this sort and is more robust to lacks of false alarms. It is calculated as follows: \(A^{\prime } = 0.5 + \left[ {\frac{(H - FA)(1 + H - FA)}{4H(1 - FA)}} \right]\).

References

  • Arita, J. T., Carlisle, N. B., & Woodman, G. F. (2012). Templates for rejection: Configuring attention to ignore task-irrelevant features. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 38(3), 580–584. doi:10.1037/a0027885.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ásgeirsson, Á. G., & Kristjánsson, Á. (2011). Episodic retrieval and feature facilitation in intertrial priming of visual search. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 73, 1350–1360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ásgeirsson, Á. G., Kristjánsson, Á., & Bundesen, C. (2014). Independent priming of location and color in identification of briefly presented letters. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 76, 40–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ásgeirsson, Á. G., Kristjánsson, Á., & Bundesen, C. (2015). Repetition priming in selective attention: A TVA analysis. Acta Psychologica, 160, 35–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bichot, N. P., & Schall, J. D. (2002). Priming in macaque frontal cortex during popout visual search: Feature-based facilitation and location-based inhibition of return. The Journal of Neuroscience, 22(11), 4675–4685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brinkhuis, M., Kristjansson, A., & Brascamp, J. (2016). Is search priming reflected in BOLD repetition suppression? Journal of Vision, 16(12), 1322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, N. B., & Woodman, G. F. (2011a). Automatic and strategic effects in the guidance of attention by working memory representations. Acta Psychologica, 137, 217–225. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.06.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, N. B., & Woodman, G. F. (2011b). When memory is not enough: Electrophysiological evidence for goal-dependent use of working memory representations in guiding visual attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(10), 2650–2664.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, N. B., & Woodman, G. F. (2013). Reconciling conflicting electrophysiological findings on the guidance of attention by working memory. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 75(7), 1330–1335. doi:10.3758/s13414-013-0529-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, C. A., & Egeth, H. E. (2016). Taming the white bear initial costs and eventual benefits of distractor inhibition. Psychological Science, 27(4), 476–485.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193–222.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Donaldson, W. (1993). Accuracy of d′ and A′ as estimates of sensitivity. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 31, 271–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Downing, P. (2000). Interactions between visual working memory and selective attention. Psychological Science, 11(6), 467–473.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Downing, P., & Dodds, C. (2004). Competition in visual working memory for control of search. Visual Cognition, 11(6), 689–703.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eimer, M., Kiss, M., & Cheung, T. (2010). Priming of pop-out modulates attentional target selection in visual search: Behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. Vision Research, 50(14), 1353–1361.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grier, J. B. (1971). Nonparametric indexes for sensitivity and bias: Computing formulas. Psychological Bulletin, 75, 424–429.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huang, L., & Pashler, H. (2007). Working memory and the guidance of visual attention: Consonance-driven orienting. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(1), 148–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kiyonaga, A., Egner, T., & Soto, D. (2012). Cognitive control over working memory biases of selection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19(4), 639–646.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kristjánsson, Á., & Campana, G. (2010). Where perception meets memory: A review of repetition priming in visual search tasks. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(1), 5–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kristjánsson, Á., & Driver, J. (2005). Priming in vision: Target repetition effects, context effects and role reversal effects. Perception, 34(Suppl.), 40c.

  • Kristjánsson, Á., & Driver, J. (2008). Priming in visual search: Separating the effects of target repetition, distractor repetition and role-reversal. Vision Research, 48(10), 1217–1232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kristjánsson, Á., & Jóhannesson, Ó. I. (2014). How priming in visual search affects response time distributions: Analyses with ex-Gaussian fits. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(8), 2199–2211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kristjánsson, Á., Saevarsson, S., & Driver, J. (2013). The boundary conditions of priming of visual search: From passive viewing through task-relevant working memory load. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(3), 514–521. doi:10.3758/s13423-013-0375-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kristjánsson, Á., Vuilleumier, P., Schwartz, S., Macaluso, E., & Driver, J. (2007). Neural basis for priming of pop-out during visual search revealed with fMRI. Cerebral Cortex, 17(7), 1612–1624.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kruijne, W., Brascamp, J. W., Kristjánsson, Á., & Meeter, M. (2015). Can a single short-term mechanism account for priming of pop-out? Vision Research, 115, 17–22. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2015.03.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lamy, D., Antebi, C., Aviani, N., & Carmel, T. (2008). Priming of pop-out provides reliable measures of target activation and distractor inhibition in selective attention. Vision Research, 48(1), 30–41. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2007.10.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lamy, D., Yashar, A., & Ruderman, L. (2010). A dual-stage account of inter-trial priming effects. Vision Research, 50(14), 1396–1401.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, H., Mozer, M. C., & Vecera, S. P. (2009). Mechanisms of priming of pop-out: Stored representations or feature-gain modulations? Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 71(5), 1059–1071. doi:10.3758/APP.71.5.1059.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maljkovic, V., & Nakayama, K. (1994). Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features. Memory & Cognition, 22(6), 657–672.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olivers, C. N. L., Peters, J., Houtkamp, R., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2011). Different states in visual working memory: when it guides attention and when it does not. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(7), 327–334. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.004.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rorden, C., Kristjánsson, Á., Pirog-Revill, K., & Saevarsson, S. (2011). Neural correlates of inter-trial priming and role-reversal in visual search. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 151.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sigurdardottir, H. M., Kristjánsson, Á., & Driver, J. (2008). Repetition streaks increase perceptual sensitivity in visual search of brief displays. Visual Cognition, 16(5), 643–658.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Soto, D., Hodsoll, J., Rotshtein, P., & Humphreys, G. W. (2008). Automatic guidance of attention from working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(9), 342–348.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, D., Kristjánsson, A., & Nakayama, K. (2005). Efficient visual search without top-down or bottom-up guidance. Perception & Psychophysics, 67(2), 239–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (2007). Do the contents of visual working memory automatically influence attentional selection during visual search? Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 33(2), 363–377. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.33.2.363.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank two anonymous reviewers for helpful suggestions on a previous version of this manuscript. This paper was supported by a Grant Nos. 152427 and 130575 from the Icelandic Research Fund (Rannís) and the Research Fund of the University of Iceland.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Árni Kristjánsson.

Ethics declarations

Statement of human rights

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Carlisle, N.B., Kristjánsson, Á. How visual working memory contents influence priming of visual attention. Psychological Research 82, 833–839 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0866-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0866-6

Navigation