Skip to main content
Log in

Destination memory and deception: when I lie to Barack Obama about the moon

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study investigates whether deceivers demonstrate high memory of the person to whom lies have been told (i.e., high destination memory). Participants were asked to tell true information (e.g., the heart is a vital organ) and false information (e.g., the moon is bigger than the sun) to pictures of famous people (e.g., Barack Obama) and, in a subsequent recognition test, they had to remember to whom each type of information had previously been told. Participants were also assessed on a deception scale to divide them into two populations (i.e., those with high vs. those with low deception). Participants with high tendency to deceive demonstrated similar destination memory for both false and true information, whereas those with low deception demonstrated higher destination memory for lies than for true information. Individuals with a high tendency to deceive seem to keep track of the destination of both true information and lies to be consistent in their future social interactions, and thus to avoid discovery of their deception. However, the inconsistency between deceiving and the moral standard of individuals with a low tendency to deceive may result in high destination memory in these individuals.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abe, N. (2011). How the brain shapes deception an integrated review of the literature. The Neuroscientist, 17(5), 560–574.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Abe, N., & Greene, J. D. (2014). Response to anticipated reward in the nucleus accumbens predicts behavior in an independent test of honesty. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(32), 10564–10572. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0217-14.2014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, A., Porter, S., ten Brinke, L., & Udala, M. (2015). Risky business: incorporating informed deception detection strategies in violence risk assessments. Archives of Forensic Psychology, 1(2), 55–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumgartner, T., Fischbacher, U., Feierabend, A., Lutz, K., & Fehr, E. (2009). The neural circuitry of a broken promise. Neuron, 64(5), 756–770. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. S., Hornstein, S., & Memon, A. (2006). Tracking conversational repetition: An evaluation of target monitoring ability. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20(1), 85–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1992). Statistical Power Analysis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1(3), 98–101. doi:10.2307/20182143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DePaulo, B. M., Kashy, D. A., Kirkendol, S. E., Wyer, M. M., & Epstein, J. A. (1996). Lying in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(5), 979–995.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, N. E., Gangi, K., Coveleski, S., Adams, A., Bernhold, Q., & Giles, H. (2016). When is it acceptable to lie? Interpersonal and intergroup perspectives on deception. [Article]. Communication Studies, 67(2), 129–146, doi:10.1080/10510974.2016.1146911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., O’Sullivan, M., & Frank, M. G. (1999). A Few Can Catch a Liar. Psychological Science, 10(3), 263–266. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • El Haj, M., Antoine, P., & Nandrino, J. L. (2016). When deception influences memory: The implication of theory of mind. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. doi:10.1080/17470218.2016.1173079.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • El Haj, M., Fasotti, L., & Allain, P. (2015). Destination memory for emotional information in older adults. Experimental Aging Research, 41(2), 204–219. doi:10.1080/0361073X.2015.1001658.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • El Haj, M., Gandolphe, M. C., Allain, P., Fasotti, L., & Antoine, P. (2015). “Forget to whom you have told this proverb”: directed forgetting of destination memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Behavioural Neurology, 2015, 215971. doi:10.1155/2015/215971.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • El Haj, M., Gely-Nargeot, M. C., & Raffard, S. (2015). Destination Memory and Cognitive Theory of Mind in Alzheimer’s Disease. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 48(2), 529–536. doi:10.3233/jad-150467.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • El Haj, M., Kessels, R. P., Matton, C., Bacquet, J. E., Urso, L., Cool, G., et al. (2016). Destination Memory in Korsakoff’s Syndrome. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 40(6), 1321–1327. doi:10.1111/acer.13070.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • El Haj, M., Omigie, D., & Samson, S. (2015). Destination memory and familiarity: better memory for conversations with Elvis Presley than with unknown people. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research, 27(3), 337–344. doi:10.1007/s40520-014-0286-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • El Haj, M., Raffard, S., Antoine, P., & Gely-Nargeot, M. C. (2015). Emotion and Destination Memory in Alzheimer’s Disease. Current Alzheimer research, 12(8), 796–801.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • El Haj, M., Raffard, S., & Gely-Nargeot, M. C. (2016). Destination memory and cognitive theory of mind in normal ageing. Memory (Hove, England), 24(4), 526–534. doi:10.1080/09658211.2015.1021257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Falkiewicz, M., Sarzynska, J., Babula, J., Szatkowska, I., Grabowska, A., & Necka, E. (2015). Explicit Instructions Increase Cognitive Costs of Deception in Predictable Social Context. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1863. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01863.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Geraci, L., McDaniel, M. A., Miller, T. M., & Hughes, M. L. (2013). The bizarreness effect: evidence for the critical influence of retrieval processes. Memory and Cognition, 41(8), 1228–1237. doi:10.3758/s13421-013-0335-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gopie, N., Craik, F. I., & Hasher, L. (2010). Destination memory impairment in older people. Psychology and Aging, 25(4), 922–928. doi:10.1037/a0019703.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gopie, N., & Macleod, C. M. (2009). Destination memory: stop me if I’ve told you this before. Psychological Science, 20(12), 1492–1499. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02472.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D., & Paxton, J. M. (2009). Patterns of neural activity associated with honest and dishonest moral decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(30), 12506–12511. doi:10.1073/pnas.0900152106.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Grober, E., & Buschke, H. (1987). Genuine memory deficits in dementia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 3(1), 13–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, C. (1997). International experiences with the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale—–a review of validation data and clinical results. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 42(1), 17–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., Ben-Zur, H., & Druch, A. (1991). The contextualization of input and output events in memory. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 53(3), 260–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., Ben-Zur, H., & Sheffer, D. (1988). Telling the same story twice: Output monitoring and age. Journal of Memory and Language, 27(1), 23–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levine, T. R., Kim, R. K., & Hamel, L. M. (2010). People Lie for a Reason: Three Experiments Documenting the Principle of Veracity. Communication Research Reports, 27(4), 271–285. doi:10.1080/08824096.2010.496334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, R. L., & Hicks, J. L. (2002). Comparisons of target output monitoring and source input monitoring. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16(7), 845–862.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel, M. A., Einstein, G. O., DeLosh, E. L., May, C. P., & Brady, P. (1995). The bizarreness effect: It’s not surprising, it’s complex. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(2), 422–435. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.21.2.422.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paulhus, D. L. (1998). Interpersonal and intrapsychic adaptiveness of trait self-enhancement: a mixed blessing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1197–1208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, J. W. (2007). PsychoPy–Psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162(1–2), 8–13. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Popliger, M., Talwar, V., & Crossman, A. (2011). Predictors of children’s prosocial lie-telling: Motivation, socialization variables, and moral understanding. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110(3), 373–392. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.05.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Serota, K. B., Levine, T. R., & Boster, F. J. (2010). The prevalence of lying in America: three studies of self-reported lies. Human Communication Research, 36(1), 2–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sip, K. E., Lynge, M., Wallentin, M., McGregor, W. B., Frith, C. D., & Roepstorff, A. (2010). The production and detection of deception in an interactive game. Neuropsychologia, 48(12), 3619–3626. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.08.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, S. A., Farrow, T. F., Herford, A. E., Wilkinson, I. D., Zheng, Y., & Woodruff, P. W. (2001). Behavioural and functional anatomical correlates of deception in humans. Neuroreport, 12(13), 2849–2853.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, S. A., & Kaylor-Hughes, C. J. (2008). Looking for truth and finding lies: the prospects for a nascent neuroimaging of deception. Neurocase: case studies in neuropsychology, neuropsychiatry, and behavioural neurology, 14(1), 68–81. doi:10.1080/13554790801992776.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ströfer, S., Noordzij, M. L., Ufkes, E. G., & Giebels, E. (2015). Deceptive Intentions: Can Cues to Deception Be Measured before a Lie Is Even Stated? PloS One, 10(5), e0125237. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0125237.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tournois, J., Mesnil, F., & Kop, J.-L. (2000). Autoduperie et hétéroduperie: un instrument de mesure de la désirabilité sociale [Self-deception and other-deception: A measure of social desirability]. European Review of Applied Psychology, 50(1), 219–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verschuere, B., Spruyt, A., Meijer, E. H., & Otgaar, H. (2011). The ease of lying. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(3), 908–911. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2010.10.023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vrij, A., Mann, S., & Fisher, R. P. (2006). An empirical test of the behaviour analysis interview. Law and Human Behavior, 30(3), 329–345. doi:10.1007/s10979-006-9014-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, C. S., Galinsky, A. D., & Murnighan, J. K. (2009). Bad drives psychological reactions, but good propels behavior: responses to honesty and deception. Psychological Science, 20(5), 634–644. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02344.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zigmond, A. S., & Snaith, R. P. (1983). The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 67(6), 361–370.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Carol Robins for linguistic assistance.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mohamad El Haj.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Ethical statement

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

All participants provided written informed consent before participation.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Haj, M.E., Saloppé, X. & Nandrino, J.L. Destination memory and deception: when I lie to Barack Obama about the moon. Psychological Research 82, 600–606 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0840-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0840-8

Navigation