Skip to main content
Log in

Congruency sequence effects and previous response times: conflict adaptation or temporal learning?

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In the present study, we followed up on a recent report of two experiments in which the congruency sequence effect—the reduction of the congruency effect after incongruent relative to congruent trials in Stroop-like tasks—was observed without feature repetition or contingency learning confounds. Specifically, we further scrutinized these data to determine the plausibility of a temporal learning account as an alternative to the popular conflict adaptation account. To this end, we employed a linear mixed effects model to investigate the role of previous response time in producing the congruency sequence effect, because previous response time is thought to influence temporal learning. Interestingly, slower previous response times were associated with a reduced current-trial congruency effect, but only when the previous trial was congruent. An adapted version of the parallel episodic processing (PEP) model was able to fit these data if it was additionally assumed that attention “wanders” during different parts of the experiment (e.g., due to fatigue or other factors). Consistent with this assumption, the magnitude of the congruency effect was correlated across small blocks of trials. These findings demonstrate that a temporal learning mechanism provides a plausible account of the congruency sequence effect.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Figure 1 roughly illustrates why the parallel episodic processing model, discussed later, produces a larger effect of expectancy-based changes in the response threshold for congruent relative to incongruent trials. The point at which evidence for a response will begin to increase and the steepness of the slope of increasing activation will vary from trial to trial within both the congruent and incongruent conditions. The evolution of the response threshold over time will also vary from trial to trial depending on the speed of responding in the last few trials. However, the figure demonstrates why, generally, congruent trials are more affected than incongruent trials by expectancies following fast congruent relative to slow incongruent responses. Congruent trials will generally benefit from a fast expectancy, but will “beat” the response threshold dip with a slow expectancy. In contrast, incongruent trials will not only benefit from slow expectancies, but will also often be fast enough to benefit from a fast expectancy.

References

  • Abrahamse, E., & Braem, S. (2015). Experience a conflict—either consciously or not (commentary on Desender, Van Opstal, and Van den Bussche, 2014). Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 179.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Blais, C., Stefanidi, A., & Brewer, G. (2014). The Gratton effect remains after controlling for contingencies and stimulus repetitions. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1207.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Boksem, M. A. S., Meijman, T. F., & Lorist, M. M. (2005). Effects of mental fatigue on attention: an ERP study. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 107–116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick, M. M., Nystrom, L. E., Fissell, K., Carter, C. C., & Cohen, J. D. (1999). Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex. Nature, 402, 179–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burle, M. S., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2005). Dynamics of facilitation and interference in cue-priming and Simon tasks. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 619–641.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Compton, R. J., Huber, E., Levinson, A. R., & Zheutlin, A. (2012). Psychophysiology, 49, 583–589.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Desender, K., Van Opstal, F., & Van den Bussche, E. (2014). Feeling the conflict: the crucial role of conflict experience in adaptation. Psychological Science, 25, 675–683.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duthoo, W., Abrahamse, E. L., Braem, S., Boehler, C. N., & Notebaert, W. (2014). The congruency sequence effect 3.0: a critical test of conflict adaptation. PLoS ONE, 9, e110462.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Egner, T. (2007). Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 380–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 480–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grice, G. R. (1968). Stimulus intensity and response evocation. Psychological Reviewer, 75, 359–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grinband, J., Savitskaya, J., Wager, T. D., Teichert, T., Ferrera, V. P., & Hirsch, J. (2011). Conflict, error likelihood, and RT: response to Brown & Yeung. NeuroImage, 57, 320–322.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grosjean, M., Rosenbaum, D. A., & Elsinger, C. (2001). Timing and reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 256–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hazeltine, E., Akçay, Ç., & Mordkoff, J. T. (2011). Keeping Simon simple: examining the relationship between sequential modulations and feature repetitions with two stimuli, two locations and two responses. Acta Psychologica, 136, 245–252.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68, 1–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A. R. (1992). The importance of intraindividual variation in reaction time. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 869–881.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jiménez, L., & Méndez, A. (2012). It is not what you expect: dissociating conflict adaptation from expectancies in a Stroop task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 271–284.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, S., & Cho, Y. S. (2014). Congruency sequence effect without feature integration and contingency learning. Acta Psychologica, 149, 60–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kinoshita, S., Forster, K. I., & Mozer, M. C. (2008). Unconscious cognition isn’t that smart: modulation of masked repetition priming effect in the word naming task. Cognition, 107, 623–649.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kinoshita, S., Mozer, M. C., & Forster, K. I. (2011). Dynamic adaptation to history of trial difficulty explains the effect of congruency proportion on masked priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 622–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohfeld, D. L. (1968). Stimulus intensity and adaptation level as determinants of simple reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76, 468–473.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lamers, M. J. M., & Roelofs, A. (2011). Attentional control adjustments in Eriksen and Stroop task performance can be independent of response conflict. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 1056–1081.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, U., Awh, E., & Laurey, P. (2003). Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 450–452.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mordkoff, J. T. (2012). Observation: three reasons to avoid having half of the trials be congruent in a four-alternative forced-choice experiment on sequential modulation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 750–757.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ollman, R. T., & Billington, M. J. (1972). The deadline model for simple reaction times. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 311–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002). Activation and suppression in conflict tasks: empirical clarification through distributional analyses. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action. Attention and performance (Vol. XIX, pp. 494–519). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, J. R. (2013a). Questioning conflict adaptation: proportion congruent and Gratton effects reconsidered. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 615–630.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, J. R. (2013b). Temporal learning and list-level proportion congruency: conflict adaptation or learning when to respond? PLoS ONE, 8, e82320.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, J. R. (2013c). The parallel episodic processing (PEP) model: dissociating contingency and conflict adaptation in the item-specific proportion congruent paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 142, 119–126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, J. R. (2014). Contingency and congruency switch in the congruency sequence effect: a reply to Blais, Stefanidi, and Brewer (2014). Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1405.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, J. R., & De Houwer, J. (2011). Now you see it, now you don’t: controlling for contingencies and stimulus repetitions eliminates the Gratton effect. Acta Psychologica, 138, 176–186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, J. R., De Schryver, M., & Weissman, D. H. (2014a). Removing the influence of feature repetitions on the congruency sequence effect: why regressing out confounds from a nested design will often fall short. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 2392–2402.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, J. R., Lemercier, C., & De Houwer, J. (2014b). Context-specific temporal learning with non-conflict stimuli: proof-of-principle for a learning account of context-specific proportion congruent effects. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1241.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, J. R., Notebaert, W., & Van Den Bussche, E. (2015). Is conflict adaptation an illusion? Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 172.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, J. R., & Weissman, D. H. (2014). Congruency sequence effects without feature integration or contingency learning confounds. PLoS ONE, 9, e0102337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 300–304.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood, J., McSpadden, M., Luus, B., & Schooler, J. (2008). Segmenting the stream of consciousness: the psychological correlates of temporal structures in the time series data of a continuous performance task. Brain and Cognition, 66, 50–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 946–958.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies on interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weissman, D. H., & Carp, J. (2013). Congruency sequence effects are driven by previous-trial congruency, not previous-trial response conflict. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 587.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Weissman, D. H., Egner, T., Hawks, Z., & Link, J. (2015). The congruency sequence effect emerges when the distracter precedes the target. Acta Psychologica, 156, 8–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weissman, D. H., Jiang, J., & Egner, T. (2014). Determinants of congruency sequence effects without learning and memory confounds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 2022–2037.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wylie, S. A., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Bashore, T. R., & van den Wildenberg, W. P. M. (2010). The effect of Parkinson’s disease on the dynamics of on-line and proactive cognitive control during action selection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 2058–2073.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Yeung, N., Cohen, J. D., & Botvinick, M. M. (2011). Errors of interpretation and modeling: a reply to Grinband. NeuroImage, 57, 316–319.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by start-up funds from the University of Michigan awarded to Daniel H. Weissman. James R. Schmidt is a postdoctoral researcher of the Research Foundation—Flanders (FWO—Vlaanderen). The authors would like to thank Tobias Egner, Antonio Freitas, Toby Mordkoff, Timothy Verstynen, and Yanni Liu for useful discussions related to this research.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to James R. Schmidt.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Schmidt, J.R., Weissman, D.H. Congruency sequence effects and previous response times: conflict adaptation or temporal learning?. Psychological Research 80, 590–607 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0681-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0681-x

Keywords

Navigation