Skip to main content
Log in

Semantic similarity between old and new items produces false alarms in recognition memory

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In everyday life, human beings can report memories of past events that did not occur or that occurred differently from the way they remember them because memory is an imperfect process of reconstruction and is prone to distortion and errors. In this recognition study using word stimuli, we investigated whether a specific operationalization of semantic similarity among concepts can modulate false memories while controlling for the possible effect of associative strength and word co-occurrence in an old–new recognition task. The semantic similarity value of each new concept was calculated as the mean cosine similarity between pairs of vectors representing that new concept and each old concept belonging to the same semantic category. Results showed that, compared with (new) low-similarity concepts, (new) high-similarity concepts had significantly higher probability of being falsely recognized as old, even after partialling out the effect of confounding variables, including associative relatedness and lexical co-occurrence. This finding supports the feature-based view of semantic memory, suggesting that meaning overlap and sharing of semantic features (which are greater when more similar semantic concepts are being processed) have an influence on recognition performance, resulting in more false alarms for new high-similarity concepts. We propose that the associative strength and word co-occurrence among concepts are not sufficient to explain illusory memories but is important to take into account also the effects of feature-based semantic relations, and, in particular, the semantic similarity among concepts.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The sample size was chosen on the basis of an a priori sensitivity power analysis (G*Power 3 software; Faul, Erdfelder, Buchner, & Lang, 2009) for F tests, since we originally planned to perform statistical analyses using ANOVAs. The power analysis revealed that our sample size is large enough to detect a significant difference (α = 0.05) between the three categories of concepts (Old, HSS, and LSS) corresponding to an effect size (\(\eta_{\text{p} }^{2}\)) as small as 0.08 with a statistical power (1 − β) of 0.80 (or \(\eta_{\text{p} }^{2}\) = 0.12 and power = 0.95). Note that the results of the ANOVAs are consistent with those reported here, but we do not include them for the sake of clarity (see “Results” section).

  2. Note that, in general, the cosine actually ranges from −1 to 1. However, in this particular case–as in most, if not all, cases in the relevant literature–cosines can take only values comprised between 0 and 1. Indeed, they are calculated from the feature vectors, which are composed solely by non-negative numbers (usually, dominance values or production frequencies).

  3. This procedure has been questioned since it can introduce a bias in the participants’ responses, termed chaining, which occurs when participants respond with associations based on a previous response rather than responding to the cue-concept. However, this bias can be reduced by appropriate instructions (see De Deyne & Storms, 2008b), and it has been shown that the vast majority of the associates produced in a continuous task were not the result of response-to-response chaining (Nelson et al., 2000).

  4. Note also that the association values were extremely low overall and, on average, they were much lower than those classically used in semantic priming studies.

  5. This variable represents the trial number vector zero-centered to remove the (possible) spurious correlation between the by-subjects random intercepts and slopes, and it accounts for potential longitudinal effects of fatigue or familiarization across participants.

  6. Note that the choice of including the familiarity predictor among all of the available psycholinguistic variables presented in Table 1 was substantiated by log-likelihood ratio tests. Indeed, the inclusion of the familiarity parameter led to the highest improvement of the model fit compared to the other psycholinguistic variables, and no other psycholinguistic variable can be included in the final model to improve its fit.

References

  • Andrews, M., Vinson, D., & Vigliocco, G. (2008). Inferring a probabilistic model of semantic memory from word association norms. In Proceedings of the 30th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 1941–1946).

  • Arndt, J., & Hirshman, E. (1998). True and false recognition in MINERVA2: explanations from a global matching perspective. Journal of Memory and Language, 39(3), 371–391. doi:10.1006/jmla.1998.2581.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baroni, M., Bernardini, S., Comastri, F., Piccioni, L., Volpi, A., Aston, G., et al. (2004). Introducing the La Repubblica corpus: a large, annotated, TEI (XML)-compliant corpus of newspaper Italian. Proceedings of LREC, 2(5), 163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B. (2012). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes, R package version 0.999999-0.

  • Bertinetto, P. M., Burani, C., Laudanna, A., Marconi, L., Ratti, D., Rolando, C., & Thornton, A. M. (2005). Corpus e lessico di frequenza dell’italiano scritto (CoLFIS) [Corpus and frequency lexicon for written Italian]. Retrieved from http://www.istc.cnr.it/grouppage/colfisEng.

  • Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2002). Fuzzy-trace theory and false memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(5), 164–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V., & Kneer, R. (1995). False-recognition reversal: when similarity is distinctive. Journal of Memory and Language, 34(2), 157–185. doi:10.1006/jmla.1995.1008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J., Yang, Y., Reyna, V. F., Howe, M. L., & Mills, B. A. (2008). Semantic processing in “associative” false memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(6), 1035–1053. doi:10.3758/PBR.15.6.1035.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan, L., Brown, N. R., Cabeza, R., & Maitson, C. (1999). False memories and semantic lexicon arrangement. Brain and Language, 68(1), 172–177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bullinaria, J. A., & Levy, J. P. (2007). Extracting semantic representations from word co-occurrence statistics: a computational study. Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 510–526. doi:10.3758/BF03193020.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cann, D. R., McRae, K., & Katz, A. N. (2011). False recall in the Deese–Roediger–McDermott paradigm: the roles of gist and associative strength. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(8), 1515–1542. doi:10.1080/17470218.2011.560272.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. (1973). The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: a critique of language statistics in psychological research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 335–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, S. E., & Gronlund, S. D. (1996). Global matching models of recognition memory: how the models match the data. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3(1), 37–60. doi:10.3758/BF03210740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cree, G. S., & McRae, K. (2003). Analyzing the factors underlying the structure and computation of the meaning of chipmunk, cherry, chisel, cheese, and cello (and many other such concrete nouns). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(2), 163–201. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.132.2.163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cree, G. S., McRae, K., & McNorgan, C. (1999). An attractor model of lexical conceptual processing: simulating semantic priming. Cognitive Science, 23(3), 371–414. doi:10.1016/S0364-0213(99)00005-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Deyne, S., Navarro, D. J., & Storms, G. (2013). Better explanations of lexical and semantic cognition using networks derived from continued rather than single-word associations. Behavior Research Methods, 45(2), 480–498.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Deyne, S., & Storms, G. (2008a). Word associations: network and semantic properties. Behavior Research Methods, 40(1), 213–231. doi:10.3758/BRM.40.1.213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Deyne, S., & Storms, G. (2008b). Word associations: norms for 1,424 Dutch words in a continuous task. Behavior Research Methods, 40(1), 198–205. doi:10.3758/BRM.40.1.198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160. doi:10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Finnigan, S., Humphreys, M. S., Dennis, S., & Geffen, G. (2002). ERP ‘old/new’ effects: memory strength and decisional factor (s). Neuropsychologia, 40(13), 2288–2304. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00113-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K. I., & Dickinson, R. G. (1976). More on the language-as-fixed effect: Monte-Carlo estimates of error rates for F1, F2, F′, and minF′. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 135–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frenck-Mestre, C., & Bueno, S. (1999). Semantic features and semantic categories: differences in rapid activation of the lexicon. Brain and Language, 68(1), 199–204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallo, D. A. (2010). False memories and fantastic beliefs: 15 years of the DRM illusion. Memory & Cognition, 38(7), 833–848. doi:10.3758/MC.38.7.833.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallo, D. A., & Roediger, H. L, I. I. I. (2002). Variability among word lists in eliciting memory illusions: evidence for associative activation and monitoring. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(3), 469–497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garrard, P., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Hodges, J. R., & Patterson, K. (2001). Prototypicality, distinctiveness, and intercorrelation: analyses of the semantic attributes of living and nonliving concepts. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18(2), 125–174. doi:10.1080/02643290042000053.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gillund, G., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1984). A retrieval model for both recognition and recall. Psychological Review, 91, 1–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, T. L., Steyvers, M., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2007). Topics in semantic representation. Psychological Review, 114(2), 211. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.211.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guida, A., & Lenci, A. (2007). Semantic properties of word associations to Italian verbs. Italian Journal of Linguistics, 19, 293–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hintzman, D. L. (1988). Judgments of frequency and recognition memory in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review, 95, 528–551.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kremer, G., & Baroni, M. (2011). A set of semantic norms for German and Italian. Behavior Research Methods, 43(1), 97–109. doi:10.3758/s13428-010-0028-x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato’s problem: the latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104(2), 211. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.104.2.211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, M. (2000). Semantic priming without association: a meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7(4), 618–630. doi:10.3758/BF03212999.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lund, K., & Burgess, C. (1996). Producing high-dimensional semantic spaces from lexical co-occurrence. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28(2), 203–208. doi:10.3758/BF03204766.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lupker, S. J. (1984). Semantic priming without association: a second look. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23(6), 709–733. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(84)90434-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McRae, K. (2004). Semantic memory: some insights from feature-based connectionist attractor networks. The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory, 45, 41–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McRae, K., & Boisvert, S. (1998). Automatic semantic similarity priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(3), 558–572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McRae, K., Cree, G. S., Seidenberg, M. S., & McNorgan, C. (2005). Semantic feature production norms for a large set of living and nonliving things. Behavior Research Methods, 37(4), 547–559.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McRae, K., Cree, G. S., Westmacott, R., & De Sa, V. R. (1999). Further evidence for feature correlations in semantic memory. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 53(4), 360. doi:10.1037/h0087323.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McRae, K., de Sa, V. R., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1997). On the nature and scope of featural representations of word meaning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126(2), 99–130. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.126.2.99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McRae, K., & Jones, M. (2013). Semantic memory. In D. Reisberg (Ed.),  The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Psychology (pp. 206–219). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • McRae, K., Khalkhali, S., & Hare, M. (2012). Semantic and associative relations in adolescents and young adults: examining a tenuous dichotomy. In The Adolescent Brain: Learning, Reasoning, and Decision Making (pp. 39–66). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, xviii. doi:10.1037/13493-002.

  • Montefinese, M., Ambrosini, E., Fairfield, B., & Mammarella, N. (2013a). Semantic memory: a feature-based analysis and new norms for Italian. Behavior Research Methods, 45(2), 440–461. doi:10.3758/s13428-012-0263-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Montefinese, M., Ambrosini, E., Fairfield, B., & Mammarella, N. (2013b). The “subjective” pupil old/new effect: Is the truth plain to see? International Journal of Psychophysiology, 89(1), 48–56.

  • Montefinese, M., Ambrosini, E., Fairfield, B., & Mammarella, N. (2014). Semantic significance: a new measure of feature salience. Memory & cognition, 42(3), 355–369. doi:10.3758/s13421-013-0365-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morey, R. D. (2008). Confidence intervals from normalized data: a correction to Cousineau (2005). Reason, 4(2), 61–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Dennis, S. (2000). What is free association and what does it measure? Memory & Cognition, 28(6), 887–899. doi:10.3758/BF03209337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Schreiber, T. A. (2004). The University of South Florida free association, rhyme, and word fragment norms. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(3), 402–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ponzetto, S. P., & Strube, M. (2007). Knowledge derived from wikipedia for computing semantic relatedness. Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research, 30, 181–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803–814.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., Watson, J. M., McDermott, K. B., & Gallo, D. A. (2001). Factors that determine false recall: a multiple regression analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(3), 385–407. doi:10.3758/BF03196177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., Guerin, S. A., & St Jacques, P. L. (2011). Memory distortion: an adaptive perspective. Trends in cognitive sciences, 15(10), 467–474.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R. M., Huber, D. E., & Marinelli, K. (1995). Effects of category length and strength on familiarity in recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(2), 267. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.21.2.267.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, D. P., & Owens, K. C. (1990). Lexical co-occurrence and association strength. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 19(5), 317–330. doi:10.1007/BF01074363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens-Adams, S. M., Goldsmith, T. E., & Butler, K. M. (2012). Variation in individuals’ semantic networks for common knowledge is associated with false memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(6), 1035–1043.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization and memory. New York: Academic Press.

  • Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Damian, M. F., & Levelt, W. (2002). Semantic distance effects on object and action naming. Cognition, 85(3), B61–B69.

  • Wright, D. B., Horry, R., & Skagerberg, E. M. (2009). Functions for traditional and multilevel approaches to signal detection theory. Behavior Research Methods, 41(2), 257–267.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yee, E., Overton, E., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2009). Looking for meaning: Eye movements are sensitive to overlapping semantic features, not association. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(5), 869–874

  • Zannino, G. D., Perri, R., Pasqualetti, P., Caltagirone, C., & Carlesimo, A. G. (2006a). Analysis of the semantic representations of living and nonliving concepts: a normative study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23(4), 515–540. doi:10.1080/02643290542000067.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zannino, G. D., Perri, R., Pasqualetti, P., Caltagirone, C., & Carlesimo, G. A. (2006b). (Category-specific) semantic deficit in Alzheimer’s patients: the role of semantic distance. Neuropsychologia, 44(1), 52–61. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.04.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zannino, G. D., Perri, R., Pasqualetti, P., Paola, M. D., Caltagirone, C., & Carlesimo, G. A. (2006c). The role of semantic distance in category-specific impairments for living things: evidence from a case of semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia, 44(7), 1017–1028. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.11.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zeelenberg, R., Boot, I., & Pecher, D. (2005). Activating the critical lure during study is unnecessary for false recognition. Consciousness and Cognition, 14(2), 316–326.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ettore Ambrosini.

Additional information

G. D. Zannino and E. Ambrosini contributed equally to this work.

Appendix

Appendix

See Appendix Table 2.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Montefinese, M., Zannino, G.D. & Ambrosini, E. Semantic similarity between old and new items produces false alarms in recognition memory. Psychological Research 79, 785–794 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0615-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0615-z

Keywords

Navigation