Skip to main content
Log in

Recruitment of intuitive versus analytic thinking strategies affects the role of working memory in a gambling task

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Previous studies found mixed results concerning the role of working memory (WM) in the gambling task (GT). Here, we aimed at reconciling inconsistencies by showing that the standard version of the task can be solved using intuitive strategies operating automatically, while more complex versions require analytic strategies drawing on executive functions. In Study 1, where good performance on the GT could be achieved using intuitive strategies, participants performed well both with and without a concurrent WM load. In Study 2, where analytical strategies were required to solve a more complex version of the GT, participants without WM load performed well, while participants with WM load performed poorly. In Study 3, where the complexity of the GT was further increased, participants in both conditions performed poorly. In addition to the standard performance measure, we used participants’ subjective expected utility, showing that it differs from the standard measure in some important aspects.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baddeley, A., & Della Sala, S. (1996). Working memory and executive control. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 351, 1397–1404.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barry, D., & Petry, N. M. (2008). Predictors of decision-making on the Iowa gambling task: Independent effects of lifetime history of substance use disorders and performance on the Trail Making Test. Brain and Cognition, 66, 243–252.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Damasio, A., Damasio, H., & Anderson, W. S. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Anderson, W. S. (1998). Dissociation of working memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 426–437.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (2000). Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain, 123, 2189–2202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bowman, C. H., & Turnbull, O. H. (2003). Real vs. facsimile reinforcers on the Iowa gambling task. Brain and Cognition, 53, 207–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buelow, M. T., & Suhr, J. A. (2009). Construct validity of the Iowa gambling task. Neuropsychology Review, 19, 102–114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cavallaro, R., Cavedini, P., Mistretta, P., Bassi, T., Angelone, S. M., Ubbiali, A., et al. (2003). Basal-corticofrontal circuits in schizophrenia and obsessive-compulsive disorder: A controlled, double dissociation study. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 437–443.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cavedini, P., Riboldi, G., Keller, R., D’Annucci, A., & Bellodi, L. (2002). Frontal lobe dysfunction in pathological gambling patients. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 334–341.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cella, M., Dymond, S., Cooper, A., & Turnbull, O. (2007). Effects of decision-phase time constraints on emotion-based learning in the Iowa gambling task. Brain and Cognition, 64, 164–169.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (Eds.). (1999). Dual process theories in social psychology. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, L., Manes, F., Antoun, N., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2003). The contributions of lesion laterality and lesion volume to decision-making impairment following frontal lobe damage. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1474–1483.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1996). The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 351, 1413–1420.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Neys, W. (2006a). Automatic-heuristic and executive analytic processing during reasoning: Chronometric and dual task considerations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1070–1100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Neys, W. (2006b). Dual processes in reasoning: Two systems but one reasoner. Psychological Science, 17, 428–433.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Nordgren, L. F., & van Baaren, R. B. (2006). On making the right choice: The deliberation-without-attention effect. Science, 311, 1005–1007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, B. D., Dalgleish, T., & Lawrence, A. D. (2006). The somatic marker hypothesis: A critical evaluation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 239–271.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. St. B. T. (1984). Heuristic and analytic processes in reasoning. British Journal of Psychology, 75, 451–458.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. St. B. T. (1996). Deciding before you think: Relevance and reasoning in the selection task. British Journal of Psychology, 87, 223–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, C. E., Kemish, K., & Turnbull, O. H. (2004). Paradoxical effects of education on the Iowa gambling task. Brain and Cognition, 54, 240–244.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. St. B. T., & Over, D. E. (1996). Rationality and reasoning. Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fellows, L. K., & Farah, M. J. (2003). Ventromedial frontal cortex mediates affective shifting in humans: Evidence from a reversal learning paradigm. Brain, 126, 1830–1837.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fernie, G., & Tunney, R. J. (2006). Some decks are better than others: The effect of reinforcer type and task instructions on learning in the Iowa gambling task. Brain and Cognition, 60, 94–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D. T. (1989). Thinking lightly about others: Automatic components of the social inference process. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 189–211). Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D. T. (1991). How mental systems believe. American Psychologist, 46, 107–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D. T. (2002). Inferential correction. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive thought (pp. 167–184). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilhooly, K. J., Logie, R. H., Wetherick, N. E., & Wynn, V. (1993). Working memory and strategies in syllogistic reasoning tasks. Memory and Cognition, 21, 115–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilhooly, K. J., Logie, R. H., & Wynn, V. (2002). Syllogistic reasoning tasks and working memory: Evidence from sequential presentation of premises. Current Psychological Research, 21, 111–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., & Kahneman, D. (Eds.). (2002). Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive thought. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinson, J. M., Jameson, T. L., & Whitney, P. (2002). Somatic markers, working memory, and decision making. Cognitive Behavioural and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 341–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hinson, J. M., Whitney, P., Holben, H., & Wirick, A. K. (2006). Affective biasing of choices in gambling task decision making. Cognitive Behavioural and Affective Neuroscience, 6, 190–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holyoak, K. J., & Spellman, B. A. (1993). Thinking. Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 265–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jameson, T. L., Hinson, J. M., & Whitney, P. (2004). Components of working memory and somatic markers in decision making. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 515–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2006). How we reason. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2003). Maps of bounded rationality: Psychology for behavioral economics. The American Economic Review, 93, 1449–1475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kerr, A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2004). Development of “hot” executive function: The children’s gambling task. Brain and Cognition, 55, 148–157.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maia, T. V., & McClelland, J. L. (2004). A reexamination of the evidence for the somatic marker hypothesis: What participants really know in the Iowa gambling task. Proceedings of the National Academy for Science USA, 101, 16075–16080.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manes, F., Sahakian, B. J., Clark, L., Rogers, R., Antoun, N., Aitken, M., et al. (2002). Decision-making processes following damage to the prefrontal cortex. Brain, 125, 624–639.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mellers, B. (2000). Choice and the relative pleasure of consequences. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 910–924.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Northoff, G., Grimm, S., Boeker, H., Schmidt, C., Bermpohl, F., Heinzel, A., et al. (2006). Affective judgment and beneficial decision making: Ventromedial prefrontal activity correlates with performance in the Iowa gambling task. Human Brain Mapping, 27, 572–587.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oaksford, M., & Chater, N. (2003). Optimal data selection: Revision, review, and reevaluation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 289–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Overman, W. H., Frassrand, K., Ansel, S., Trawalter, S., Bies, B., & Redmond, A. (2004). Performance on the IOWA card task by adolescents and adults. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1838–1851.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oya, H., Adolphs, R., Kawasaki, H., Bechara, A., & Damasio, A. R. (2005). Electrophysiological correlates of reward prediction error recorded in the human prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 102, 8351–8356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reverberi, C., Rusconi, P., Paulesu, E., & Cherubini, P. (2009). Response demands and the recruitment of heuristic strategies in syllogistic reasoning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Colchester), 62, 513–530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanfey, A. G., Loewenstein, G., McClure, S. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2006). Neuroeconomics: Cross-currents in research on decision-making. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 108–116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sloman, S. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 3–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sloman, S. (2002). Two systems of reasoning. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive thought (pp. 379–396). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slovic, P., Finucane, M., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2002). The affect heuristic. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive thought (pp. 397–420). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 645–665.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tranel, D., Bechara, A., & Denburg, N. L. (2002). Asymmetric functional roles of right and left ventromedial prefrontal cortices in social conduct, decision-making, and emotional processing. Cortex, 38(4), 589–612.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turnbull, O. H., Evans, C. Y., Bunce, A., Carzolio, B., & O’Connor, J. (2005). Emotion based learning and central executive resources: An investigation of intuition and the Iowa gambling task. Brain and Cognition, 57, 244–247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • von Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1944). Theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wason, P. C., & Evans, J. St. B. T. (1975). Dual processes in reasoning? Cognition, 3, 141–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Windmann, S., Kirsch, P., Mier, D., Stark, R., Walter, B., Gunturkun, O., et al. (2006). On framing effects in decision making: Linking lateral versus medial orbitofrontal cortex activation to choice outcome processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1198–1211.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Germano Rossi for his statistical help, and Simona Salomone, Giulia Mattavelli and Selenia Di Bari for their help in collecting the data. This study was supported by two FAR grants to PC and CP and by a PRIN 2005 grant to CP.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marta Gozzi.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gozzi, M., Cherubini, P., Papagno, C. et al. Recruitment of intuitive versus analytic thinking strategies affects the role of working memory in a gambling task. Psychological Research 75, 188–201 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-010-0296-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-010-0296-1

Keywords

Navigation