Introduction

Consistent differences in cognitive performance between breastfed and infant formula-fed infants have been reported [1].

In contrast to the content of infant formula feeding at least up to the end of the twentieth century, breast milk contains long-chain (LC) polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFAs), which are important structural components of the brain. Hence, it has been hypothesized that the beneficial effect of breastfeeding on cognitive performance may be the result of a long-term effect of LC-PUFAs.

Some observational studies have reported that consumption of oily fish, which is known to be rich in docosahexaenoic acid (DHA), an essential n-3 LC-PUFA, during pregnancy is positively associated with improved cognitive performance of the child [25]. However, results of randomized controlled trials of maternal supplementation with n-3 LC-PUFAs during pregnancy and lactation, compared with a control supplementation, have been inconsistent [6]. This is probably due to the relatively small sample sizes (n < 100) of the trials and, consequently, their limited statistical power to detect small differences. In addition, in these trials, there is no information about the actual effect of n-3 LC-PUFAs supplementation on the fatty acid composition in breast milk. Another way to investigate the effect of n-3 LC-PUFAs on cognitive performance is to enrich infant formulas with LC-PUFAs. Meta-analyses of randomized clinical trials of infant formulas supplemented with LC-PUFAs did not demonstrate a clear or consistent benefit to cognitive performance by supplementing the formulas with LC-PUFAs [7, 8]. A recent review suggested that sample size, genetic polymorphisms, sex, source of supplement, dose, timing of supplementation, duration of supplementation, compliance with treatment and the selection of the test of assessment of cognitive performance outcomes may be the factors responsible for these inconsistent results [9].

In contrast, two observational studies suggest that higher levels of n-3 LC-PUFAs, directly measured in breast milk, and higher ratios between n-3 and n-6 PUFAs in colostrum, play a beneficial role in cognitive performance measured at 14 months and 6.5 years of age [10, 11]. We would like to proceed in line with these two studies and, with our observational birth cohort study, investigate the association between LC-PUFA composition of infant feeding and cognitive performance, measured as school performance, at 12 years of age.

Due to the possible differences in the metabolic capacity to synthesize n-3 LC-PUFAs [12] based on sex, we will separate the analyses for boys and girls in our study. Furthermore, in contrast to the previous studies, in our study, children who were never breastfed served as reference, because at the time when the children were born (1996–1997), infant formulas did not contain n-3 LC-PUFAs in the Netherlands [13], and therefore, this reference group has never received n-3 LC-PUFAs in their first months of life.

Subjects and methods

Study design and setting

The current study was embedded in the Prevention and Incidence of Asthma and Mite Allergy (PIAMA) Study, a population-based Dutch birth cohort study. Details of this study are described elsewhere [14]. In short, pregnant women were recruited from the general population during their first antenatal visit. Their children were born in 1996–1997 and have been followed up to the age of 12 years. The study protocol was approved by the medical ethics committees of the participating institutes, and all parents gave written informed consent. In a subsample of the mothers, breast milk samples were collected around the child’s age of 3 months. At the age of 12 years, information on school performance was obtained. For this observational study, we identified two subgroups from among the children with known school performance: children who were never breastfed (n = 277) and children from a subsample of mothers who collected a breast milk sample for fatty acid analyses (n = 157).

Fatty acid composition of breast milk

In the current study, children who were never breastfed served as reference, because at the time the children were born (1996–1997), infant formulas did not contain n-3 LC-PUFAs in the Netherlands [13], and therefore, this reference group had never received DHA and EPA in their first months of life.

Data on breastfeeding initiation were assessed by a questionnaire at age 3 months. We collected information on fatty acid composition of the breast milk from the group of breastfed children.

The fatty acid composition of the breast milk was determined by gas–liquid chromatography, according to Foreman-van Drongelen et al. [15]. The majority of the breast milk samples (77 %) were obtained between 11 and 18 weeks after delivery. Further details of breast milk collection and analysis have been reported elsewhere [16]. Fatty acids were measured as a weight percentage (wt%) of the total fat content. Fatty acids of main interest in relation to cognitive functioning were DHA (22:6[n-3]), EPA (20:5[n-3]), AA (20:4[n-6]), the sum of n-3 LC-PUFAs (≥20), DHA/AA ratio and EPA/AA ratio.

School performance

The parents received a questionnaire when the children were ~12 years old with which information concerning school performance was collected. As indicator for school performance in this study, we used test results of a standardized school achievement test, the Cito test and teacher’s advice for the level of secondary school. In the Netherlands, secondary education starts at age 12 and consists of several levels which prepare children for vocational education, higher professional education or university. In order to assign children to the most appropriate secondary education levels, first of all, an academic achievement test is administered at the end of their primary education at age 11–12 years. The most commonly used test for this purpose is the (Cito) test which is developed by the National Institute for Educational Measurements (Cito, Centraal Instituut voor Toetsontwikkeling) [17]. The test includes questions concerning spelling, math, study skills and world studies. Together the performance scales result in a standardized score between 501 and 550. On average, a pre-vocational secondary education is recommended to children with a Cito-test score between 501 and 536, a senior general secondary education is recommended to children with a score ranging from 537 to 544, and a pre-university education is recommended to children with a score between 544 and 550 [17].

Another way to measure school performance is the teacher’s school-level advice. The teacher bases his advice on the Cito-test score as well as the entire school career, skills and characteristics of the child. It is common that children receive the advice to participate in a combination class of two education levels (e.g., combination of senior general and pre-university education). This results in a seven-point scale concerning teacher’s school-level advice. Both the Cito-test score and the school teacher’s advice were measured when the children were 12 years of age and were reported by the parents. Both Cito-test score and the school teacher’s advice are indicators of cognitive performance.

Potential confounders and covariates

Maternal smoking during pregnancy (any smoking by the mother during pregnancy ≥4 weeks after onset of pregnancy), maternal age at the time of birth in years, sex of the child, birth weight of the child in grams and gestational age in weeks were assessed using questionnaires administrated during pregnancy and the first year of the child’s life.

Parental education level was measured by means of a questionnaire completed by the parents when the children were 1 year old. Both parents were asked to report their educational level separately on a seven-point answering scale (1 = completed primary school; 7 = completed university). Maternal and paternal education was classified into three categories: high, intermediate and low education. Type of child care in the first year (no child care outside home, small-scale child care, day care center attendance) was assessed by a parental report.

Children’s mental health at 12 years of age was assessed using the five-item version of the Mental Health Inventory (MHI-5). The MHI-5 includes questions concerning feeling nervous, calm, happy, depressed and anxious. The scores of five items (ranging from 1 to 5) were added and transformed into a total scoring ranging from 0 to 100. Scores of 60 or less indicate children to be psychological unhealthy, whereas scores of 61 or more indicate good psychological health [18]. Fish consumption (times per week) was assessed by self-report of the parents, and pubertal development was assessed by self-report of the children at the age of 11.

Statistics

Participant characteristics are presented as means with standard deviations or percentages. The association of fatty acid composition of breast milk with school performance was analyzed with multiple linear regression analyses and multinomial logistic regression. Infant milk feeding fatty acid composition as the independent variable had 3 categories, no n-3 LC-PUFAs (infant formula), “low” in n-3 LC-PUFAs and “high” in n-3 PUFAs, where high was defined as above the median of 0.49 wt% (see online resource 1). The dependent variables were the two indicators for school performance, namely the Cito-test score and school teachers advice. We first analyzed the association between fatty acid composition of breast milk and Cito-test scores. The results are reported as regression coefficients and 95 % CI. The regression coefficient for breast milk can be interpreted as the mean differences (Cito-test score) between low or high fatty acid content and the reference group of no fatty acids (infant formula). In addition, we assessed the linear association between n-3 LC-PUFAs and Cito test by restricting the analyses to breastfed children only and without categorizing n-3 LC-PUFAs. Next we analyzed the association between fatty acid composition of breast milk and school teacher’s advice. The results are reported as OR and 95 % CI. We adjusted all models for variables that were associated with our determinant and outcome, namely parental education level, smoking during pregnancy and child care. The following variables were not associated with the determinant and outcome: gestational age, MHI-5, fish intake of the child, maternal age, birth weight and breastfeeding duration. The analyses were repeated for different types of PUFAs, i.e., DHA, EPA, AA and the ratio of EPA/AA and DHA/AA as independent variables.

We performed analyses stratified for sex to identify different associations within strata.

All statistical analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS (version 20 for windows).

Results

Children who were never breastfed more often had a smoking mother, lower educated parents, a lower gestational age and attended day care less compared with breastfed children (Table 1). Furthermore, formula-fed children had a lower Cito-test score and got a lower school advice of the teacher compared with breastfed children (Table 2).

Table 1 Characteristics of the study population
Table 2 Descriptive statistics of the school performance of the study population

Tables 3 and 4 show the association between LC-PUFAs and Cito-test scores as the indicator of school performance. After full adjustment, children who received breast milk with a relatively high amount of DHA had a 1.43 (95 % CI −0.56; 3.42) marginally higher Cito-test score than formula-fed children (Table 3). After full adjustment, girls who received breast milk with a relatively high amount of n-3 LC-PUFAs had a 2.33 (95 % CI −0.32; 4.97) points marginally higher Cito-test score than formula-fed girls (Table 4). This was also the case for the single n-3 LC-PUFA DHA (β = 2.96 points higher, 95 % CI 0.24; 5.67). A relatively high amount of all other single n-3 LC-PUFAs and larger DHA/AA and EPA/AA ratios were associated with a higher Cito-test score in girls. There was no association between any of the LC-PUFAs and Cito-test scores in boys (Table 4). Also, there were no associations between a relatively low amount of LC-PUFAs in breast milk and Cito-test scores. Tables 5 and 6 show the association between LC-PUFAs and Cito-test scores within the breastfed children. These tables show similar results to Tables 3 and 4: after full adjustment in girls, for each percentage point of higher total n-3 LC-PUFA content of breast milk, the Cito-test score was 4.55 points (95 % CI 0.43; 8.66) higher. This was also the case for the single n-3 LC-PUFA DHA (β = 7.09 points higher, 95 % CI 0.91; 13.27) and the DHA/AA ratio (β = 2.33 points higher, 95 % CI 0.02; 4.63). For boys, there was no association between LC-PUFAs and Cito score.

Table 3 Difference in test scorea between children with low or high LC-PUFAs compared to formula-fed children (no LC-PUFAs)
Table 4 Difference in test scorea between children with low or high LC-PUFAs compared to formula-fed children (no LC-PUFAs)
Table 5 Association between LC-PUFAs in breast milk and test scorea
Table 6 Association between LC-PUFAs in breast milk and test scorea, for boys and girls

The association between LC-PUFAs and teacher’s school advice regarding the suitable school level for a child as the indicator of school performance showed a similar pattern (online resource 2). A higher proportion of girls who received breast milk with a relatively high amount of total n-3 LC-PUFAs, DHA, EPA, DHA/AA ratio and EPA/AA ratio get a higher school-level advice compared to formula-fed girls (online resource 3). However, after full adjustment, the associations attenuated to nonsignificant. There were no associations between LC-PUFAs and teacher’s school advice for boys (online resource 4) and in children who received a relatively low amount of n-3 LC-PUFAs in breast milk.

Discussion

Summary of main findings

This study shows that girls receiving breast milk with a relatively high content of DHA had a significantly better school performance at age 12 years after adjustment for confounding factors than formula-fed girls. A similar tendency was observed for the breast milk content of total n-3 LC-PUFA, EPA, AA and the ratio of DHA/AA and EPA/DHA, but these associations were not statistically significant after adjustment for potential confounders. There was no association between LC-PUFA content of breast milk and school performance in boys.

Comparison with literature and underlying mechanism

It has been observed before that breast feeding is associated with better cognitive performance but the mechanism is unclear. However, n-3 and n-6 LC-PUFAs are important components of the cell membranes and, therefore, essential in the formation of new tissue, including neurons and glial cells [19, 20]. In addition, PUFA may have a neuroprotective role, making nervous tissue less susceptible to damage [21]. Moreover, EPA, DHA and AA can act as secondary messengers and also induce release of acetylcholine and noradrenaline. These neurotransmitters are involved in memory and learning and may play a role during development [22, 23]. Taken together, all these factors, brain components, neuroprotection and neurotransmission, may play a role in the association between LC-PUFA content of breast milk and cognitive performance at 12 years of age. So far only the association between cognitive performance and LC-PUFA content of breast milk in very young children has been investigated [10, 11]. Gustafsson et al. [11] showed in a very small sample (n = 28) that high colostrum LC-PUFAs levels were associated with better cognitive performance at the age of 6.5 years. However, breast milk samples after 1 and 3 months did not disclose an association between LC-PUFA content and cognitive performance in this study. In addition, a recent study reported that higher levels of n-3 LC-PUFAs and higher ratios between n-3 and n-6 LC-PUFAs in breast milk were positively associated with cognitive performance in children at 14 months of age [10]. We have extended these findings by showing that a high content of LC-PUFA in breast milk is associated with better school performance among girls at 12 years of age.

Although the previous mentioned studies did not assess sex differences in the association between breast milk fatty acids and cognition, earlier studies have assessed whether the association between supplementation of LC-PUFAs in infant formulas and cognition differs according to the child’s sex. Our results are in line with these results reporting a beneficial effect for girls [24, 25]. Although it has been shown that fatty acid metabolism is different between men and women [26], there is yet no satisfactory explanation for these sex differences in children. We can only speculate about possible mechanisms. First, the metabolic difference between men and women may also be present in children, although metabolic differences seem to be driven by sex hormones [26] which may not apply to infants. Second, environmental influences have been shown to increase at later age [27]. These influences may be larger for boys than girls. Third, there is an age-specific development of cognitive function [28], and therefore, at the age of 12, our outcome measurement may have been more sensitive for girls than for boys.

Methodological considerations

Strengths of this study include the prospective study design and the availability of LC-PUFA content of breast milk. However, some limitations have to be considered. We performed multiple comparisons with related exposure variables, which increase the chance that a statistically significant estimate is found. No adequate method for adjustment for multiple comparisons exists. Therefore, we think it is important to interpret our findings based on effect size and consistency.

Because we obtained only one sample of breast milk, it is not known whether the fatty acid composition in the breast milk sample reflects the fatty acid composition of the breast milk of the entire lactation period. However, breast milk fatty acid composition reflects long-term food intake by the mother [29] and predicts infant DHA status [30]. Moreover, we found no correlation between breast milk fatty acids and the time between delivery and when the samples were obtained. If misclassification occurred, this misclassification is, however, unlikely to be differentially associated with later school performance; that is, it is unlikely that all of the mothers with a lower than their usual content of n-3 LC-PUFAs in their milk on the day they collected the sample will have children with a higher school performance at age 12 years.

We collected data on fatty acid composition of breast milk in a subsample of the PIAMA study. The mothers who were willing to donate a milk sample had higher education and were less often smokers than those who did not breastfeed. However, we consider it to be unlikely that the association between breast milk fatty acid content and school performance at age 12 is different in the group that we studied than in the total study population. Also, because of the subsample we used, we were only able to compare two groups of breastfeeding (by median split). As indicated by the width of the confidence intervals, our sample size was sufficient to perform analyses stratified by sex, and adjustment for the main confounders, but our sample size was not sufficient to create more than 2 groups.

Adjustment for the confounders resulted in a large attenuation of the estimates, especially in the analyses where infant formula was used as the reference group. Estimates between crude and adjusted analysis within the breastfed group were more similar, as confounding by lifestyle and sociodemographic factors determining choice of infant milk feeding was not an issue in this analysis. Thus, residual confounding by lifestyle factors can never be ruled out, especially in the analyses where formula-fed children were used as the reference. On the other hand, in unadjusted analyses, an association between n-3 LC-PUFAs and cognition was seen in boys and girls, while after adjustment, only associations in girls were seen. Residual confounding should then include sex-specific confounders. Sex-specific confounding is unlikely, and therefore, residual confounding might not completely explain the reported association.

Implications

We showed, at least in girls, that infant feeding is important for long-term cognitive performance at least till the age of 12. Other studies found that better cognition in childhood predicts better cognition in adulthood. As cognition is also associated with socioeconomic status and because of the strong associations between socioeconomic status and health, perhaps improving cognition in childhood may eventually benefit population health.

Therefore, our findings call for novel, well-powered RCTs that verify the effect of LC-PUFA supplementation or intake on the breast milk content in relation to child cognitive performance.

Conclusion

Although a large part of the association between infant milk feeding and cognition seems to be explained by sociodemographic and lifestyle-related factors, a relative high content of n-3 PUFAs, especially DHA, in breast milk is associated with better school performance in 12-year-old girls but not in boys.