Introduction

Anthropogenic global change poses a host of novel challenges to species worldwide (Grimm et al. 2008; Martin et al. 2010; Pautasso et al. 2010). Two important components of direct or indirect human impact on animals are the urbanization of natural environments and the displacement of animals into non-native ranges (Vitousek et al. 1997; Lambin and Meyfroidt 2011). Interestingly, invasive species distributions are often centered around cities and other anthropogenically modified habitats (Pyšek et al. 2020). This might simply be a result of the more frequent transportation to, and release within, human-modified habitats due to human activity and global inter-connectedness that is inherent to the concept of invasive populations (Hulme 2009; Gaertner et al. 2017; Padayachee et al. 2017). However, recent evidence suggests that the invasiveness of species may be enhanced by prior adaptation to urban environments before they are subsequently released into novel urban ranges, bolstering a population’s ability to establish and spread (Hufbauer et al. 2012; Borden and Flory 2021).

Behavioral flexibility is a key mechanism that can enable animals to cope with rapid human-induced environmental changes and can even allow them to thrive in heavily modified habitats like cities (Candolin and Wong 2012; Sol et al. 2013; van Baaren and Candolin 2018). Commonly reported behavioral shifts in response to novel anthropogenically driven conditions, such as urban landscapes or extra-limital introductions, include increases in activity (e.g., Pintor et al. 2008; Pintor and Sih 2009; Philpott et al. 2019), aggression (e.g., Pintor et al. 2008; Evans et al. 2010; Kralj-Fišer and Schneider 2012), boldness (e.g., Atwell et al. 2012; Batabyal et al. 2017; Hewes and Chaves-Campos 2018; Damas-Moreira et al. 2020; Mazza et al. 2020), and tendency to explore (e.g., Thompson et al. 2018; Damas-Moreira et al. 2019; Oliveira et al. 2020), but can vary between taxa and locations. Research on why these behavioral shifts occur indicates that urban-adapted and/or invasive populations can achieve higher growth rates and reproductive output in anthropogenically modified habitats (Peach et al. 2008; Pintor and Sih 2009; Sargent and Lodge 2014; Putman and Tippie 2020; Thawley and Kolbe 2020) by using novel ecological opportunities (e.g., González-Bernal et al. 2012; González-Bernal et al. 2016; Thawley and Kolbe 2020) and outcompeting native species (e.g., Pintor and Sih 2009; Damas-Moreira et al. 2020) while benefitting from reduced predation pressure (Brownscombe and Fox 2013; Eötvös et al. 2018; Westrick et al. 2019) and higher resource availability (Lowry et al. 2013; Tryjanowski et al. 2015; Hradsky et al. 2017); however, many questions remain regarding how these behavioral adaptations arise.

Similar behaviors appear to be beneficial for animals in urbanized or invasive populations (e.g., increased boldness; Atwell et al. 2012; Damas-Moreira et al. 2020; Mazza et al. 2020), yet the mechanism driving these behavioral tendencies is often unclear. Anthropogenically derived phenotypic change may be driven by prior adaptation in the donor habitat (Hufbauer et al. 2012; Borden and Flory 2021), rapid evolution in the novel environment (Whitney and Gabler 2008; Baxter-Gilbert et al. 2019), or realization through developmental plasticity and behavioral flexibility (e.g., Hudson et al. 2017; Hewes and Chaves-Campos 2018; Mazza et al. 2020). Furthermore, exactly when trait divergence occurs during ontogeny is largely unknown (e.g., Brodin et al. 2013; Gruber et al. 2017). This question is especially relevant when different ontogenetic stages of an animal exhibit different morphologies and use diverse ecological niches and habitats (Werner 1988; Kelleher et al. 2018). This is more commonplace than one may expect—approximately 80% of all animals, including insects and amphibians, have these types of complex life cycles (Werner 1988).

We aimed to study evolutionary and ontogenetic divergence of a behavioral trait (i.e., boldness; herein defined as an animal’s propensity to exhibit risk-taking behavior) known to differ among populations of an urban-invasive amphibian, the Guttural Toad (Sclerophrys gutturalis). A recent study by Baxter-Gilbert et al. (2021) found that adult toads are bolder in urban habitats than they are in natural areas across native and invasive ranges. This study gives rise to the questions of how (i.e., through evolution or plasticity) and when (i.e., at what age or developmental stage) these differences arise. In a common garden experiment, we raised tadpoles produced from wild adults captured from rural-native, urban-native, and urban-invasive populations (i.e., origin populations). We then quantified the tadpoles’ boldness (i.e., latency to leave a hide after a standardized simulated predator attack; Baxter-Gilbert et al. 2021) every 10 days across development to investigate whether differences in boldness between habitat types are genetically fixed, and if so, when do these differences arise ontogenetically. We expected that (1) all tadpoles increase in boldness as they age, due to an increased energy requirement with larger body size, as well as the well-established link between boldness and foraging activity (Wilson and Krause 2012b; Urszán et al. 2015a, b; Kelleher et al. 2018), and (2) urban-invasive tadpoles will be consistently bolder across ontogeny than rural-native tadpoles, with urban-native tadpoles being intermediary, given the reported coupling of boldness across life stages in anurans (Wilson and Krause 2012b; Kelleher et al. 2018). If prediction (2) is supported, then this would also suggest that prior adaptation to invade urban habitats is already present in the tadpole stage of this species (Hufbauer et al. 2012; Borden and Flory 2021).

Methods

Study species, sampling sites, and collection of adults

The Guttural Toad is a large bufonid with a maximum reported snout-vent length (SVL) of 140 mm, that has a wide distribution across much of sub-Saharan Africa (du Preez et al. 2004; Telford et al. 2019). Tadpoles are aquatic and develop over the course of 6 to 10 weeks, after which metamorphosis is complete and they leave the water with a terrestrial body form (du Preez et al. 2004; Channing et al. 2012). This toad is a habitat generalist that is encountered across a range of grassland and forest ecosystems commonly associated with natural wetlands and waterbodies (du Preez et al. 2004; Channing et al. 2012). Yet, it also has a proclivity for synanthropic behavior and can be found in anthropogenic landscapes (e.g., urban/suburban) where they will exploit anthropogenic water bodies such as garden ponds for breeding habitat (du Preez et al. 2004; Vimercati et al. 2017).

The species has successfully established invasive populations on the islands of Mauritius and Réunion and near the city of Cape Town, South Africa (Telford et al. 2019; Baxter-Gilbert et al. 2020). A study by Telford et al. (2019) showed that all invasive populations came from the port city of Durban, South Africa. In the case of the invasive population near Cape Town, eggs and tadpoles are presumed to have been unintentionally introduced via a consignment of aquatic ornamental plants in 1998 (De Villiers 2006; Measey et al. 2017). This urban-invasive Cape Town population has since shown rapid adaptation to the drier climate in the Cape region in the adult stage, altering their behavior, physiology, and reproductive strategy (Vimercati et al. 2018, 2019; Madelaire et al. 2020; Barsotti et al. 2021).

For the purposes of our study, we collected reproductively sized adult toads from November 2019 to February 2020. We chose two locations to collect toads from natural-native origin populations, hereafter referred to as “Durban Rural” (from sites at 29° 51′ 31″ S, 30° 43′ 18″ E and 29° 28′ 17″ S, 31° 13′ 25″ E), which were areas with little human modification that are 20–60 km from the city of Durban. We also collected toads from two urban-native origin populations, hereafter referred to as “Durban Urban” (29° 50′ 55″ S, 31° 00′ 30″ E and 29° 49′ 08″ S, 30° 56′ 37″ E), which were from suburban areas within Durban. Lastly, we collected toads from a single urban-invasive origin population, hereafter referred to as “Cape Town” (34° 01′ 29″ S, 18° 26′ 03″ E) from the suburb of Constantia, located near Cape Town, where an invasive population of Guttural Toads had become established. The Durban Rural sites consisted of wetlands with a lake or a series of ponds that showed little anthropogenic modification or signs of low levels of use by humans (e.g., nature reserves and infrequent recreational fishing) and had mostly native plant communities. The Durban Urban sites had artificial water bodies (e.g., ornamental ponds with non-native fish species, ditches, or fountains), frequent anthropogenic structures (e.g., buildings and roads), and high levels of human presence. Similarly, the Cape Town site also had many garden ponds, buildings, roads, and high levels of human presence.

Husbandry of adults and breeding protocols

After collection, adults were brought to a greenhouse located at the University of KwaZulu-Natal Westville Campus. In this facility, toads were housed by sampling location and sex in large plastic tubs (1.1 m [L] × 1.3 m [W] × 0.50 m [H]; n = 10) filled with ~50 mm of soil and another ~50 mm of leaf litter over the soil. Water was provided in plastic bowls ad libitum. We ensured the soil was moist, but not saturated, and fed toads 2–5 adult crickets (Acheta domesticus) every other day.

Toad breeding was carried out from December 2019 to March 2020 using individuals that were available in the greenhouse at the time. Thus, clutches were not deposited synchronously, but rather in clumped batches over the course of this ~3-month period. Every toad was used only once for breeding. To initiate mating behavior, and subsequent oviposition and fertilization, we used a 1:20 solution of the synthetic gonadotrophin leuroprorelin acetate—“Lucrin” (Lucrin Depot, Abbott) and Ringer’s solution. We injected females with 0.666 mL and males with 0.333 mL (Hamilton et al. 2005) at ~17:00 h. After injection, pairs of males and females were placed inside separate plastic containers (260 × 410 mm) filled with 40 mm of aged tap water and left undisturbed overnight. The next morning at ~9:00 h, we checked the containers for fertilized clutches, removed the adults from the containers, and replaced the water inside these containers with clean, aged tap water to a height of approximately 150 mm. We regularly replaced the water inside these containers and made sure clutches were well aerated. Over the next few days, we followed embryonic development closely. Typically, 1 day after the clutch was laid, most embryos had already hatched (i.e., Gosner stage 20; Gosner 1960). At this point, individuals were assigned a tadpole age of 0 days, allowing us to track age in days for the entirety of our common garden experiment.

Common garden experiment

We housed tadpoles in large mesocosms (large plastic tubs; 1.1 m [L] × 1.3 m [W] × 0.5 m [H]; n = 30) filled with 600 L of tap water placed halfway below workbenches inside the facility, so that one half of a mesocosm was shaded and the other half received sunlight through the roof panels. We let the water inside the mesocosms age for a week, before adding ~1 L of water from a standing water tank to each mesocosm to induce zoo- and phyto-plankton establishment. This water tank was located inside the facility (i.e., preventing access from toads and fish) and had a well-established community of aquatic vegetation and algae within it. After another week, we added 50 g of rabbit chow (Rabbit Pellets, Westerman’s Premium) to supply the plankton with additional nutrients (Semlitsch and Boone 2009). At an age of 3 days, we separated 1000 enumerated tadpoles from one clutch (i.e., full siblings) and translocated them to a mesocosm that had been prepared for their arrival approximately 1 month before and contained a well-established and abundant zoo- and phyto-plankton community.

Boldness assay

At the age of 10 days, and then every subsequent 10 days (Table 1), we haphazardly selected 20 tadpoles from each mesocosm for testing. If we were not able to conduct the assay on the specific day the tadpoles were scheduled for, we conducted the assay on the next possible day (see Table 1). We did not assess Gosner stage (Gosner 1960) of each tadpole as this would have required additional handling. Generally, developmental stages of individual tadpoles varied greatly within a clutch (e.g., between Gosner stages 25 and 30 for younger clutches and Gosner stages 30 and 41 for older clutches; Gosner 1960). However, for older tadpoles, we ensured that only tadpoles that had not developed past Gosner stage 41 (Gosner 1960) were used, as strong morphological and physiological changes might affect behavior in subsequent developmental stages (Dodd and Dodd 1976). We used 190 [L] × 300 [W] mm plastic containers filled with 20 mm of aged tap water as boldness arenas. Above the boldness arenas, we installed digital video cameras (recording at a frame rate of 25 fps and a resolution of 101.38 kP), that allowed us to remotely record the tadpole behavior (i.e., without a researcher present). Before the start of each boldness assay, tadpoles were tested for swimming performance (Mühlenhaupt et al. 2021) and were introduced to the boldness arenas immediately afterwards. Before starting the simulated predator attack, we documented the water temperature in the boldness arenas using a digital thermometer (Control Company, product code: 4052; ± 0.1°C). Using the tip of a red silicon spatula, a single researcher (MM) chased the tadpoles for 10 s before placing a circular hide (i.e., a white opaque plastic cup with a diameter of 116 mm with a 50 [W] × 30 [H] mm exit hole cut into the side) over them. The hide, while it was surrounding the tadpole, was then centered within the arena and the exit hole aligned to be visible by the camera. For the next 20 min, we recorded tadpole behavior. From these video recordings, we scored whether tadpoles left the hide during the duration of recording (binary variable) and documented the time (s) it took the tadpoles to leave the hide (i.e., latency to leave the hide). For the latter behavior, we considered a tadpole had left the hide when it had passed through the exit hole with the vent exposed and the tip of the snout facing perpendicular to the hide. Video scoring was carried out by one observer (MM) who was blind to the origin populations of tadpoles. Tadpoles that did not leave the hide were assigned the maximum assay time (1200 s; as per Riley et al. 2017; Damas-Moreira et al. 2019; Baxter-Gilbert et al. 2021). This protocol closely followed the boldness assay used by Baxter-Gilbert et al. (2021). We measured the same variables and began each boldness assay with a standardized “mock predation” event (i.e., chasing the tadpoles for 10 s with a rubber spatula) to ensure comparability of our results with those of Baxter-Gilbert et al. (2021). Furthermore, we ensured that boldness, as quantified here, was not conflated with general activity by simulating a risky situation through the “mock predation” event at the beginning of each assay (see Réale et al. 2007; Kelleher et al. 2018).

Table 1 The number of tadpoles whose boldness was assayed in our study, separated by age and origin population. Numbers in parentheses denote the total number of clutches the tadpoles belonged to. For Durban Rural, 3 and 7 clutches were produced from toads collected in sampling location 1 or 2, respectively, and for Durban Urban, 6 and 6 clutches from sampling location 1 or 2, respectively (see “Methods”). Please note that due to the staggered breeding of adults over a period of 3 months, as well as the onset of COVID-19 pandemic restrictions in South Africa in March 2020, sample sizes are lower for higher ages as we were not able to continue the data collection after this time

After the boldness assay, we returned the tadpoles to their respective mesocosms. Thus, we cannot rule out the possibility that any individual tadpole was also sampled for a boldness assay at a different age, and we acknowledge that recurrent behavioral testing can influence tadpole behavior in later trials (Urszán et al. 2015a). However, given the high initial number of tadpoles per mesocosm (n = 1000), there is a low likelihood that many tadpoles were repeatedly used for testing and thus, we conducted statistical analysis assuming non-repeated measures.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were carried out using R (Version 4.0.1; R Core Team 2020). Before analyzing our data, we explored the data set following Zuur et al. (2010) and did not find any unexplainable outliers. We verified the assumptions of normality and homoscedasticity of residuals for all models prior to interpretation and present data as predicted means ± standard error (SE) in the text. The significance level (α) was set at 0.05 for all models. The water temperature during the boldness trials and the age of tadpoles were significantly correlated (confirmed by using the “lm” and “Anova” function in the R “stats” package; R Core Team 2020: F1, 2493 = 11.12, p < 0.01). Therefore, we decided to exclude temperature from our models because these two factors are confounded. However, due to our staggered breeding protocol (see “Husbandry of adults and breeding protocols”), younger and older tadpoles were tested at the end of the experiment, so seasonal variation in temperature likely did not affect our results strongly.

First, we investigated the proportion of tadpoles that did not leave the hide during the 20-min trial. The variability in this behavioral trait was very low (overall less than 1% of tadpoles did not exit the hide; Table 2). Thus, we present a summary of this behavioral trait, but it was not analyzed statistically. Instead, we used the tadpole’s latency to leave the hide (s) as our boldness metric and, before analyses, we rank-transformed these values because they were strongly positively skewed (Riley et al. 2017; Damas-Moreira et al. 2019). We used a linear mixed effect model (LMM; using the “lmer” function in the R package “lme4”; Bates et al. 2015) with our boldness metric as the response variable that included the fixed effects of tadpole origin population (categorical: Cape Town, Durban Rural, Durban Urban), age (continuous), and initially included the interaction between origin population and age. This LMM also included a random intercept and slope of mesocosm number to control for dependency in our data due to relatedness and shared housing for individuals from the same tub, and a random intercept of parentage location to control for dependency in our data due to similarities between specific study sites where the parents were collected. Post hoc, we tested for an interaction effect using “emtrends” in the R package “emmeans” (Lenth et al. 2018) and did not find any significant effects (all pcorr > 0.16; p-values were corrected using a “Tukey” adjustment). Thus, we removed the interaction term and reran the LMM to allow interpretation of main effects.

Table 2 A summary of the number of tadpoles that did not leave the hide during the 20-min boldness assays separated by age and origin population. Numbers in parentheses denote the specific percentage (%) of tadpoles that did not leave the hide as calculated from the number of tadpoles that did not leave the hide divided by the total number of tadpoles assayed at that age and specific origin population (see Table 1)

We also ran a separate LMM investigating differences in the boldness of tadpoles that were 40/41 days old among the different origin populations. This LMM had our boldness metric as the response variable, included tadpole origin population (categorical: Cape Town, Durban Rural, Durban Urban) as a fixed factor, and a random intercept of mesocosm number and parentage site. We chose this age because tadpoles had sufficient time to develop and possibly diverge in boldness, sample size was still reasonably high (Table 1), and, around this age, Guttural Toad tadpoles are known to begin emergence as toadlets (du Preez et al. 2004; Channing et al. 2012). Furthermore, our previous work has indicated that tadpoles from different origin populations show differences in developmental rate (Mühlenhaupt et al. 2021). Thus, with higher age (e.g., 50 or 60 days), our results might be biased towards slow-developing Durban Urban tadpoles because fast-developing Durban Urban tadpoles might have already exceeded Gosner stage 41 (Gosner 1960).

Following analysis of both LMMs, we conducted post hoc multiple comparisons between tadpole origin populations using the R package “emmeans” and p-values generated for these comparisons were “Tukey” adjustment-corrected using a Monte Carlo method to produce “exact” Tukey corrections (Lenth et al. 2018).

Results

The proportion of tadpoles that did not leave the hide during our boldness assay was very low, with less than 1% of tadpoles remaining in the hide during the trials (Table 2). Tadpoles became less bold (i.e., shyer) as they aged, but boldness did not differ between origin populations (Table 3, Fig. 1). Similarly, at the age of 40/41 days, tadpole boldness did not differ among origin populations (Table 4, Fig. 2).

Table 3 (a) Output from the linear mixed effects model examining differences in boldness scores (i.e., rank-transformed latency to leave the hide) over the first 60 days of development in Guttural Toad tadpoles. The interaction between tadpole origin population and age was not significant, so it was removed, and the model was re-run. Model estimates (β) of fixed effects are presented with their corresponding standard errors (SE), Satterthwaite-approximated degrees of freedom (df), and t-values. Variance estimates (σ2) are supplied for residuals and random effects, and all significant values (p < 0.05) are bolded. Levels for tadpole origin population are given in parentheses following the variable name. (b) We also present post hoc multiple comparisons of boldness between all tadpole origin populations and, in this case, p-values (pcorr) were corrected using a “Tukey” adjustment (Lenth et al. 2018)
Fig. 1
figure 1

Guttural Toad tadpole boldness (i.e., rank-transformed latency to leave the hide after a standardized simulated predation attempt) as they aged for each origin population as predicted from our linear mixed effect model (Durban Rural = green with solid line, Durban Urban = black with dotted line, Cape Town = blue with dashed line). Predicted data points are shown with corresponding linear regression lines and 95% confidence intervals. The blue triangle on the left corresponds with the directionality of the trait (i.e., low values correspond with bold tadpoles and vice versa)

Table 4 (a) Output from the linear mixed effects model examining differences in Guttural Toad tadpole boldness (i.e., rank-transformed latency to leave the hide) at the age of 40/41 days. For fixed effects, model estimates (β) are presented with their corresponding standard errors (SE), Satterthwaite-approximated degrees of freedom (df), and t-values. For residuals and random effects, variance estimates (σ2) are supplied. Levels for tadpole origin population are given in parentheses following the variable name. (b) We also present post hoc multiple comparisons of boldness between all tadpole origin populations, and, in this case, p-values (pcorr) were corrected using a “Tukey” adjustment (Lenth et al. 2018)
Fig. 2
figure 2

Guttural Toad tadpole boldness (i.e., rank-transformed latency to leave the hide after a standardized simulated predation attempt) at the age of 40/41 days. Depicted are boxplots of predicted data from a linear mixed effect model testing for differences between Durban Rural (green), Durban Urban (black), and Cape Town (blue) tadpoles. The thick horizontal line in the boxplots corresponds with the predicted median, the boxes represent the second and third quartile ranges, and the whiskers encompass the minimum and maximum of the data. We did not find any significant differences in boldness among tadpoles from different origin populations (Table 2). The blue triangle on the left depicts the directionality of the trait (i.e., low values correspond with bold tadpoles and vice versa)

Discussion

Contrary to our predictions, tadpole boldness did not differ among rural-native, urban-native, and urban-invasive origin populations, and decreased with tadpole age in our common garden experiment. Given the clear divergence in boldness among natural and urban adult Guttural Toads (Baxter-Gilbert et al. 2021), this study gives valuable insight in how urban-derived boldness in Guttural Toads may or may not arise. Notably, this would suggest that the increased boldness observed in adult urban toads is either a result of experience or plasticity at either the tadpole or adult stage (i.e., requiring exposure to actual urban/rural ecosystems), or if it is heritable and fixed, then the phenotype is not expressed until after metamorphosis. Within the scope of this study, since bolder phenotypes are not uniquely arising in the larval stage of urbanized Guttural Toads, we would assert that prior-adaptation is not enhancing the species’ invasion success (Hufbauer et al. 2012; Borden and Flory 2021), with respect to tadpole boldness. In the context of the recent surge in studies examining rapid evolution in urban and invasive populations, our study results demonstrate a break with many significant findings on larval stages, suggesting substantial gaps in our understanding of the mechanisms and timing by which these phenotypic differences arise, and support the recommendation for more robust methodologies (see Lambert et al. 2020).

Our common garden experimental design gives us the ability to demonstrate strong evidence that Guttural Toad tadpoles are not expressing heritable or innate differences in their boldness between populations (Lambert et al. 2020). These findings are contrary to previous research which showed heritable differences in tadpole boldness between mainland and island populations of Common Frogs (Rana temporaria) that were carried over to the adult stage (Brodin et al. 2013). In Common Frogs, the bolder phenotype of island populations was presumed to be the result of a disperser-dependent founder effect through bolder, more exploratory adults colonizing the new islands that rose above sea level approximately 350–800 years ago (Brodin et al. 2013), which is similar to the increased levels of boldness seen in invasive range-front (i.e., expanding) adult Cane Toads (Rhinella marina) in Australia after less than 100 years (Gruber et al. 2017). In the context of Guttural Toads, however, it is more likely that a population around the area of Durban already existed before the city was founded and became urbanized during the city’s founding and growth (Baxter-Gilbert et al. 2021). Furthermore, the Cape Town population of Guttural Toads was likely established through tadpoles or eggs, both of which are non-dispersive life stages (De Villiers 2006; Measey et al. 2017). Thus, a disperser-dependent founder effect is unlikely in the urban and urban-invasive populations of the Guttural Toad, emphasizing the context-dependent nature of behavioral divergence between populations. Similarly, a previous study showed that urban-native and urban-invasive tadpoles do not differ in swimming performance and underlying morphological traits compared to rural-native tadpoles in the Guttural Toad (Mühlenhaupt et al. 2021), suggesting that predation pressure might not be reduced in urban and/or invasive tadpole habitats.

Although our findings suggest that urbanized or invasive Guttural Toad tadpoles are not exhibiting heritable increases in boldness, the drivers of the observed differences in adult boldness between urban and rural adults (Baxter-Gilbert et al. 2021) remain unknown. One possibility is that phenotypic differences could be the result of developmental effects experienced during the tadpole stage, which may be habitat-dependent for their expression in tadpoles and are carried over and expressed during post-metamorphic stages. Tadpoles are well known to show high levels of plasticity in many traits as a response to different ecological factors (Richardson 2001; Relyea 2002; Van Buskirk 2002; Relyea 2004) and differences in stress response between urban, agricultural, and rural Common Toad (Bufo bufo) tadpoles are seen to be a result of phenotypic plasticity (Bókony et al. 2021). Furthermore, more active, explorative, and bolder phenotypes in Lake Frog (Pelophylax ridibundus) tadpoles are seen to be carried over to the adult stage (Wilson and Krause 2012b) and if these traits arise through plasticity, then variable environments would be required to trigger their expression. As such, due to the controlled nature of our common garden experimental design, if the bolder phenotypes are a result of plasticity, then it is not surprising that we were unable to detect them in this study. A reciprocal transplant experiment might help to elucidate what role phenotypic plasticity of boldness and the evolution of plasticity in this trait play in shaping the bolder urban phenotype of adult toads (Baxter-Gilbert et al. 2021). For example, plasticity in boldness might have evolved only in urban populations, and then may only be observable if the correct stimuli are present during development to trigger a shift in boldness (e.g., Iraeta et al. 2006; Kaiser et al. 2019).

Conversely, differential expression of boldness between adults but not tadpoles might be the result of decoupled evolution between life stages in Guttural Toads (Collet and Fellous 2019). Decoupling of traits has recently been shown in the invasive African Clawed Frog (Xenopus laevis; Kruger et al. 2021). Adults from the periphery of the invasive range in France show a distinct dispersal-mediating phenotype compared to adults from the core (Louppe et al. 2018; Padilla et al. 2019). However, when reared from the egg to the post-metamorphic frog, those differences were not apparent, indicating that differences in adult traits emerge well after metamorphosis (Kruger et al. 2021). Similarly, in a previous study, we presented data indicating that adult Guttural Toads are larger in urban areas but did not find differences in growth rates of tadpoles when raised in a common garden environment (Mühlenhaupt et al. 2021). The amphibian larval stage is typically more sedentary and adapted for foraging and growth, whereas the adult stage is more mobile and adapted for dispersal and reproduction (Wilson and Krause 2012a). Combined with the far-reaching changes in the environment from the aquatic to the terrestrial habitat (Dodd and Dodd 1976), differential selective forces are likely acting on both stages and decoupling of traits across metamorphosis could enhance phenotypic match in the environment experienced by the specific ontogenetic stage (Wilbur 1980; Collet and Fellous 2019; Goedert and Calsbeek 2019). Therefore, if selection has favored increased boldness in urbanized and/or invasive Guttural Toad populations, then the expression of this trait may well be evolutionarily decoupled between the sedentary tadpole life stage and the active, dispersing, and mate-searching adult life stage.

Beyond an evolutionary mechanism (e.g., local adaptation), the presence of bolder urbanized adult Guttural Toads (Baxter-Gilbert et al. 2021), but the absence of such a trait in tadpoles, may be a result of circumstance, due to behavioral flexibility, learning, and/or acclimatization (e.g., Levey et al. 2009; Wright et al. 2010; Couchoux and Cresswell 2012; Vincze et al. 2016; Evans et al. 2019). For example, several phenotypic traits of adult invasive Cane Toads are strongly influenced by prior experience and developmental environment, such as thermal acclimation (McCann et al. 2014) or antipredator response (Hudson et al. 2017). Novel environments, such as cities or the extra-limital ranges of introduced populations, often require animals to phenotypically respond rapidly to novel or stressful ecological conditions (Grimm et al. 2008). Behavioral flexibility and learning represent some of the quickest mechanisms to adaptively match novel selective regimes (Ghalambor et al. 2007; Lande 2009; Fox et al. 2019). Such scenarios can be seen with Northern Mockingbirds (Mimus polyglottos), which are capable of rapidly learning and identifying individual humans and adjusting their flight distance based on their prior experience with a specific person (Levey et al. 2009), or experience-driven decreases in risk aversion by Burrowing Owls (Athene cunicularia) towards humans (Cavalli et al. 2018). Therefore, it follows that Guttural Toads in urban habitats could have their behavioral traits incrementally shaped through differential experiences, rather than evolutionary mechanisms, resulting in the expression of a bolder adult phenotype in these populations.

Our finding that tadpoles became less bold with increasing age provides clear evidence that boldness levels can change across ontogeny, even within a given life stage, which provides important insights for future research looking to disentangle where the divergence in adult boldness between urban and rural populations occurs (Baxter-Gilbert et al. 2021). This finding was contrary to our predictions because, as we expected that older and larger tadpoles would spend more time foraging, and as there often is a correlation between foraging activity and boldness, we expected our tadpoles to also be bolder as they aged (Wilson and Krause 2012b; Urszán et al. 2015a, b; Kelleher et al. 2018). However, our results aligned with previous research on Common Toad tadpoles, similarly reared in a common garden environment, which were shown to decrease their activity levels (which can correlate with boldness; Kelleher et al. 2018) across larval development (Kurali et al. 2018). Thus, it is likely that tadpoles early in development (after the consumption of the yolk sac; Gosner 1960) show increased foraging activity and boldness to enhance resource acquisition, which then lessen over time due to consequent growth and energy storage (Ramamonjisoa et al. 2019; Davenport et al. 2020). This change in behavioral traits across ontogeny, potentially related to energetics, will also play an important role in interpreting future research on how traits, like boldness, change between life stages and how this may or may not relate to adaptive behavioral shifts associated with urbanization or biological invasions.

Overall, we showed that there is no innate difference in boldness among Guttural Toad tadpoles from rural-native, urban-native, and urban-invasive ranges, despite the well-documented divergence of boldness in adults (Baxter-Gilbert et al. 2021). Our study highlights the importance of studying the ontogenetic and environmental context of behavioral shifts in novel environments. We encourage researchers, interested in rapid evolution in urban and novel environments, to carefully choose experimental approaches that identify the underlying mechanisms driving population divergence (see Lambert et al. 2020). Although this study was able to provide evidence against one potential driver of human-induced behavioral change (i.e., fixed, heritable traits that persist across life stages) within this study system, there remains a number of alternative mechanisms and means from which phenotypes can arise that will require future research.